Teaching Critical Thinking in Psychology: A Handbook of Best Practices

(ff) #1

Dana S. Dunn & Randolph A. Smith


166


Learning from Exemplars

How can students be taught to discern logical, evidence-based psychological research from


poorly designed or flawed efforts? The best course is to have them read high quality


exemplars from the literature. We will expose psychology majors, of course, to a variety of


high quality examples through class work, library research, and the discipline-based


textbooks they read. Teachers can supplement these examples by intentionally presenting


the details of high quality studies to students. Presenting inconclusive, questionable, or


hard-to-interpret studies or descriptive (i.e., noncausal) findings is also a good idea so that


students can learn to distinguish the reliable, scientific wheat from the chaff. Fine books


that promote this sort of approach to critical thinking are available (e.g., Marton, 2006;


Meltzoff, 1998; Stanovich, 2007).


Alternatively, students can learn from one another by reading and critiquing work pro-


duced by other students. Various undergraduate journals exist, and the material published


in them is readily understood by undergraduate students (Ware, Badura, & Davis, 2002).


More to the point, evaluating the strengths and weaknesses of such work can be a more


gentle entry into critical reading than immediately tackling the work of professional psy-


chologists, especially famous ones.


Evaluating Reading Through Writing

For students, a typical part of evaluating what they read involves taking notes about it,


whether on index cards or in a notebook (paper or computer). This general activity can


be improved upon by having students create a “reader’s guide” concerning whatever


topic they are researching (Henderson, 2000). This guide is a topical précis containing


a content outline (e.g., history of topic, major topics, theories and methods), a list of


Table 14.1. Claims Categorized: Some Typical Types of Arguments Offered in Psychology


Theory advancement or modification – claims made to extend or revise what is known (e.g., “Our


findings qualify the role of some automatic processes in working memory.”).


New ideas or improvements – claims that share novel approaches (e.g., “We developed a new paper


and pencil measure for assessing implicit attitudes.”).


Challenges to prevailing assumptions – claims aimed at overturning existing theories explanations


of results (e.g., “The results from our studies confirm that positive emotions elicit distinct and


different behavioral responses compared to negative emotions.”).


Utility and application of results – claims indicating that results can be used to address some


problem constructively (e.g., “We found that academic intrinsic motivation can be encouraged


among elementary schoolchildren who are at risk for learning delays.”).


Contesting established judgment – claims offered to counter expectations or assumptions with


supporting evidence (e.g., “Despite the ubiquity of media advertising, our series of stud-


ies reveals that the connection between persuasive messages and actual purchase is extremely


limited.”).


Source: Adapted from Spellman et al. (2007).

Free download pdf