Teaching Critical Thinking in Psychology: A Handbook of Best Practices

(ff) #1

Paul C. Smith & Kris Vasquez


220


adopt an analytical attitude in advance of hearing the argument, may be useful (Pizarro &


Bloom, 2003). But if you catch them unprepared, and their first reactions are emotional


ones, it will be exceptionally difficult for them to backtrack and consider the arguments in


a carefully detached way.


Conclusion

It is ironic that the critical thinking skills we attempt to teach to our students seem to fail


where they are needed most, but it is also no accident. Students resist a detached, scientific


evaluation of certain beliefs for compelling reasons, including the need to preserve common-


sense epistemology and social accountability. We believe that without careful attention to


the conditions that promote this resistance, our goal of increasing critical thinking will be


difficult to achieve. However, despite the challenges outlined here, we also believe that it is


possible to make progress, even in the face of immovable objects and irresistible forces.


References

Anderson, C. A., Lepper, M. R., & Ross, L. (1980). Perseverance of social theories: The role of


explanation in the persistence of discredited information. Journal of Personality and Social


Psychology, 39 , 1037–1049.


Brooks-Gunn, J., Han, W.-J., & Waldfogel, J. (2002). Maternal employment and child cognitive


outcomes in the first three years of life: The NICHD study of early child care. Child


Development, 73, 1052–1072.


Carey, S., & Smith, C. (1993). On understanding the nature of scientific knowledge. Educational


Psychologist, 28 , 235–251.


Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (1993). The role of anomalous data in knowledge acquisition:


A theoretical framework and implications for science instruction. Review of Educational


Research, 63 , 1–49.


Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (1998). An empirical test of a taxonomy of responses to anomalous


data in science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 35 , 623–654.


Gibbard, A. (1990). Wise choices and apt feelings. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.


Glaser, R. (1992). Expert knowledge and the processes of thinking. In D. F. Halpern (Ed.),


Enhancing thinking skills in the sciences and mathematics (pp. 63–75). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.


Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral


judgment. Psychological Review, 108 , 814–834.


Haidt, J., Rosenberg, E., & Hom, H. (2003). Differentiating diversities: Moral diversity is not like


other kinds. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33 , 1–36.


Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases.


Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.


Kohlberg, L. (1981). The philosophy of moral development: Moral stages and the idea of justice. Vol. 1:


Essays on moral development. San Francisco: Harper & Row.


Lord, C. G., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1979). Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The


effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social


Psychology, 37 , 2098–2109.

Free download pdf