Teaching Critical Thinking in Psychology: A Handbook of Best Practices

(ff) #1

17


Critical Thinking: Needed Now More Than Ever


The truth is that neuroscientists are dealing with correlational data, and if some area is


activated during some psychological process, it could mean any number of things: It may


be the sole locus of the operation; it may have been disinhibited by some other area that is


of equal or greater importance even though that other area is not as active; it may be nec-


essary for the mental operation but not sufficient unless other areas, including areas of


lesser activity, are also involved; it may contain neurons that operate less efficiently than


do those in other areas, and that therefore must consume more energy; and so on. What


this means is that it is hard to know whether the image in a brain scan actually gives us a


“picture” of the neurodynamics of the operation in question. All of these caveats fly out


the window, however, when the media jump on some finding, especially if it has to do


with something that captures the public’s imagination, such as sex differences or spiritual


experiences.


The issues I have discussed, both methodological and conceptual ones, are not aca-


demic. Increasingly, brain scans are being used in ways that those doing the original


research may never have anticipated or desired. For instance, brain scans are starting to be


introduced as evidence in court cases to argue for diminished responsibility and are being


promoted as “lie detectors” in criminal cases; one commercial company has even begun


offering a brain-scan test for deception, and the Defense Department and CIA have


reportedly invested millions in neuroimaging technologies that might be used in law


enforcement or intelligence. But because of the normal variability among people in their


brain responses, innocent but highly reactive people may be mislabeled “guilty” by these


seemingly scientific tests, just as has happened in the use of the polygraph and other methods


that assume the existence of universal biological responses in emotional states.


Thus we need to teach our students to think critically in part so they can separate the


wheat from the chaff in our own field of psychology. If they intend to become thera-


pists, they will understand that good therapy and an appreciation of research are not


mutually exclusive. If they intend to use the techniques of neuroscience, they will under-


stand that science is not merely a matter of technique; it is rooted in an attitude toward


evidence and interpretation—an attitude that requires critical thinking at every stage of


the process.


Barriers to Critical Thought in the Culture

There is also a larger picture to consider, for the existence of uncritical thinking within our


own field is occurring in a culture that is often distrustful of science and relativistic in its


thinking about scientific and nonscientific claims. We hear this distrust and relativism


when our students say, “Well, that’s just my opinion,” as if all opinions were created


equal—end of discussion.


Relativism has found expression in the renewed debate about the teaching of creation-


ism, now repackaged as intelligent design, in our public schools, as a counterpoint to


evolution. I would certainly never fault a person for having religious faith, and I think a


good argument can be made that there is no necessary conflict between evolution and


religion as broadly construed. No less a scientist than Francis Collins, Director of the

Free download pdf