Teaching Critical Thinking in Psychology: A Handbook of Best Practices

(ff) #1

Carole Wade


18


Human Genome Project, is a Christian who believes that God used the mechanism of


evolution to create the world, including human beings. The problem occurs when people


misunderstand the meaning of the term theory in science, and when they assume that


intelligent design and evolution have equal standing as scientific theories. Such misunder-


standings are common among students.


As Collins (2005) wrote in Time magazine, nearly all working biologists, whether reli-


gious believers or not, accept that the principles of variation and natural selection explain


how multiple species evolved from a common ancestor over very long periods of time, and


agree that these processes are sufficient to explain the rich variety of life forms on the


planet. Indeed, evolution is the very basis of modern biology, and plays an increasing


role in psychological theory as well. The processes of evolution are plain for everyone to


see every time a virus or bacterium becomes resistant to a drug, as have flu viruses over just


the past five years. Evolution is evident in the adaptive changes that have occurred during


the 20th century in many more complex species, such as the peppered moth in England


and the rock pocket mouse in Arizona (Nachman, Hoekstra, & D’Agostino, 2003;


Young & Musgrave, 2005). And evolution is evident in the recent comparative analyses of


human and chimpanzee genomes. In contrast, as Steven Pinker (2005) has observed, the


idea of intelligent design runs smack into the inconvenient facts that the retina is installed


backward, that the male seminal duct hooks over the ureter like a garden hose snagged on


a tree, and that, when we are cold, goose bumps uselessly try to warm us by fluffing up


long-gone fur. Nonetheless, because so many people misunderstand what science is, in a


2005 Pew poll, 38% of respondents favored replacing evolution with creationism in the


science curriculum. As conservative commentator Charles Krauthammer (2005), who is


generally sympathetic toward the role of religion in American life, has asked, in an essay


lamenting the public confusion of faith with science, “How many times do we have to


rerun the Scopes ‘monkey trial’?”


Religious ideologies, then, can get in the way of critical thinking about science,


including psychological science. And so can political ideologies, so shrill these days on


both the right and the left. Because of such ideologies, reactions to psychological find-


ings, especially those that challenge conventional beliefs, such as findings on sexual


orientation, gender, abstinence-only sex education, and the emotional effects of abor-


tion, often have little to do with a study’s scientific merits. Scientifically literate students


need to know this.


Other social trends also decrease the ability (or willingness) of people to think critically.


One, as Frank Cioffi (2005) noted in the Chronicle of Higher Education, is that the national


language of “debate” has become cheapened. Many television programs, and political


commentators like Fox News host Bill O’Reilly, whose commentaries contain an average


of 8.88 instances of name-calling per minute (as noted in The Week, May 18, 2007, p. 16),


have reduced public discourse to a verbal food fight, in which the person who shouts the


loudest and says the nastiest things wins. Thus it is little wonder that students often fail to


understand the very concept of intellectual argumentation, or the value of coming up with


counterarguments.


In sum, the scientist–practitioner gap, ideological intrusions into science, relativistic ways


of thinking in the culture, uncritical responses to the biotechnical revolution, and other

Free download pdf