Teaching Critical Thinking in Psychology: A Handbook of Best Practices

(ff) #1

63


The Challenge of Assessing Critical Thinking



Experiencing the appropriate humility that grows from realizing that you can only

have an incomplete handle on reality.


Helping our students not become jerks is a justifiable goal for our activities in the


classroom.


One of the controversial aspects of our early FIPSE discussions involved the drive to


craft the perfect definition of critical thinking. The philosophers wanted to talk about the


attributes of the critical thinker, typically expressed in traits. For example, Paul and Elder’s


(2002, p. 18) concept of “critical thinking in the deep sense” exemplifies this approach.


This “all-or-none” approach makes me uncomfortable because I regularly falter in my


ability as a critical thinker. However, I am much more comfortable construing critical


thinking as a set of behaviors, leading us to ...


Big Idea #3: We should regard critical thinking as a “state,” not a “trait.”


A framework that emerged from our FIPSE group (Halonen, 1986) is one that still drives


much of the design of the teaching in my own classroom. This model (see Figure 6.1)


targets the essential characteristics of how to facilitate critical thinking. The model


acknowledges that students do not arrive in psychology classrooms as blank slates with


regard to their understanding of behavior. They have a store of facts, beliefs, assumptions,


and values that serve as the foundation from which they construct “personal theory” about


behavior. As teachers, we present external stimuli that we think and hope will engage


students. It is perhaps easiest to get them to engage critically when the external stimulus


promotes cognitive disequilibrium, a force described long ago by Jean Piaget as a primary


driver of learning. By knocking students cognitively off balance, they will engage in critical


thinking to restore their balance.


In the beginning of the student’s journey in psychology, the external stimulus needs to


be a whopper. For example, in my intro class recently, I introduced something I had


heard on the news the morning of my class that I had confidence would be the perfect


external stimulus to engage discussion. A morning news team had a spirited discussion


about sagging, the art of wearing your pants at half-mast without them falling down. One


newscaster confidently concluded, “The lower the pants, the lower the IQ.” My students


were appalled at the audacity of the claim. Not only did we debate the truthfulness of the


claim, but it was a good way to begin the important discussion about “correlation is not


causation.” The conversation was vigorous and laid the groundwork for the develop-


mental progression predicted within the model. Challenging the truths promulgated by


newscasters in such a personal way should assist students in developing a more critical


orientation. According to the FIPSE model, as students improve in their critical thinking


skills, the external event that triggers critical thinking can become ever more subtle and


nuanced.


This model also launched my personal fascination with the progression of “novice to


expert” in the work articulating outcomes in psychology that would follow (see Bosack,


McCarthy, Halonen, & Clay, 2004; Halonen et al., 2003, as examples). It is profoundly


satisfying to isolate a skill set and describe its evolution from primitive beginnings to

Free download pdf