9781118041581

(Nancy Kaufman) #1
Revising Probabilities 547

is simply (.5)(280,000) (.5)(0) $140,000. Remembering that the expected
profit without the test is $120,000, we find the test has an EVI of 140,000 
120,000 $20,000. This test is much less valuable than the perfect seismic test
examined earlier. Nonetheless, if the test is inexpensive enough (costs less than
$20,000), the partners should elect it.

REVISING PROBABILITIES


In many situations, the decision maker possesses potentially valuable informa-
tion, but in a form that is not readily usable. Typically, the decision maker must
ask: How does this piece of information alter my assessment of an uncertain
event? Does it make the event more or less likely? By how much should I revise
the event’s probability? A considerable body of research has studied the ways
in which individuals make probabilistic predictions. The overwhelming evi-
dence from these studies is that one’s intuition often is a poor guide when it
comes to probability assessment and revision. (See the discussion of intuitive
prediction later in the chapter.) Fortunately, some basic results in probability
provide a formal method for handling this task.
To illustrate the method, suppose the partners lack the seismic record
listed in Table 13.1. Instead, they have the following summary information
about the accuracy of the seismic test. The vendor of the test certifies that, in
the past, sites that were actually wet tested “good” three-quarters of the time and
dry sites tested “bad” two-thirds of the time. In mathematical terms, we have
Pr(GƒW) 3/4 and Pr(BƒD) 2/3. As before, the partners assess a 40 percent
chance that the site is wet based on their information prior to the seismic test,
Pr(W)  .4.
How can the partners derive Pr(WƒG) and Pr(WƒB), the two key proba-
bilities they need to solve their decision tree? The most direct way is to com-
pute the table of joint probabilities in Table 13.1. Consider the calculation of
one such joint probability, Pr(W&G), appearing in the upper-left corner of
the table. The partners reason as follows: According to their prior judgment,
the site is wet 40 percent of the time. A wet site can be expected to test good
three-quarters of the time. Therefore, the site is both wet and good three-
quarters of 40 percent of the time, or 30 percent of the time. In algebraic
terms, we have Pr(W&G) (3/4)(.4)  .3. What is the probability that the site
is dry and (falsely) tests good? Since Pr(D)  .6 and only one-third of dry sites
test good, the joint probability is Pr(D&G) (1/3)(.6)  .2. The other joint
probabilities are computed in similar fashion. The basic result is that any joint
probability can be expressed as the product of a prior probability and a con-
ditional probability. In the first calculation, we made use of the result

Pr(W&G)Pr(GƒW)Pr(W). [13.1]

c13TheValueofInformation.qxd 9/26/11 11:02 AM Page 547

Free download pdf