New Scientist 2018 sep

(Jeff_L) #1
10 | NewScientist | 8 September 2018

NEWS & TECHNOLOGY


Timothy Revell

DO COMPUTERS know what we
are thinking? To find out, a team
at the University of California,
Berkeley, has created a set of
gruelling tests that probe AI’s
progress in understanding the
world. None has passed the tests
yet, but one got very close.
The tests examine theory of
mind – the ability to reason about
another’s beliefs – and are inspired
by experiments in psychology.
Each consists of a short paragraph
describing a scenario involving
people – for example, Sally hides
a marble and then Anne moves it
to a new location.
Passing the test revolves around
identifying first and second-order
beliefs. A first-order belief is
working out what someone else
thinks, answering a question like
“where does Sally think the marble
is?”, for example. A second-order
belief is what someone thinks
someone else is thinking, for
example, “where does Anne think
Sally thinks the marble is?”.
Children are normally able
to correctly identify first-order
beliefs by around age 3, but it isn’t
until age 6 or 7 that they can do

the same for second-order beliefs.
Aida Nematzadeh, who led the
work and is now at Google’s AI lab
at DeepMind, and her colleagues
generated 10,000 scenarios and
associated questions that tested
theory of mind via first and
second-order beliefs. They then
put them to four state-of-the-art
AIs. None managed to achieve a
passing score, which was set at
95 per cent. Most humans should
be able to score 100 per cent.
The highest achiever was an AI
called RelNet, produced by Adam
Santoro and his colleagues at
DeepMind, which scored 94.3 per
cent. The other three AIs managed
scores of between 82 and 94 per
cent (arxiv.org/abs/1808.09352).
However, the results were fairly
fickle. Inserting an unrelated
sentence with no bearing on the
tested situations was enough to
bamboozle the AIs. All of them
dropped their scores by between
5 and 20 per cent, suggesting
they weren’t properly grasping
the meaning of the text.
Though no AI has yet displayed
theory of mind, they are
improving at an impressive rate.
Only two years ago, Facebook
produced a series of tests that

would examine AI’s ability to
answer questions about the world.
None tested could handle the task
at the time, but some can now
pass with few mistakes.
A machine with theory of mind
could prove useful, says Alan
Wagner at Georgia Tech Research
Institute in Atlanta. “An AI agent
teaching assistant might be able
to reason about a student’s false
beliefs related to a course, for

example, that the final exam
is next week when in fact it is
tomorrow, and in doing so be
better suited to help.”
Another example is self-driving
cars, says Johannes Bjerva at
the University of Copenhagen,
Denmark. An AI-driven car with
theory of mind might realise that
the driver of another car hasn’t
seen a person in the road, and honk
its horn to warn them, he says.
However, even if an AI can pass
these tests, it may still not have
theory of mind, says Wagner. ■

AI takes ‘marble’


theory-of-mind test


EYEEM/ALAMY STOCK PHOTO

“ If we take action now,
some coral reefs may still
exist a hundred years in
the future”

REPEATING an experiment 47 years
after it was originally carried out has
revealed some rare good news about
corals: some species appear to have
become significantly better at
surviving temperature increases.
In 1970, marine zoologist Steve
Coles collected three species of coral
from a reef in Kane‘ohe Bay, Hawaii.
When he put them in a chamber and

Some corals are


beginning to


beat the heat


gradually heated the water, he found
that they couldn’t tolerate increases
of 1 to 2°C. They started to bleach,
ejecting the colourful algae that live
within them. Without these, coral die.
Overall, no corals from one species
survived, and survival rates were 40
per cent and 5 per cent for the others.
But Coles, at the Hawaii Institute of
Marine Biology, got a different result
when he repeated the experiment
using the same species of coral from
the same area decades later. When
he raised temperatures by the same
amount, it took a few days longer for
the corals to begin bleaching, and more

than half survived (PeerJ, doi.org/cthn).
“That was a big surprise,” says Coles.
The finding is in line with anecdotal
observations from reefs that some
corals seem to be surviving bleaching.
Coles doesn’t know how they do it,
but other research suggests some
corals adapt to higher temperatures
by associating with species of algae
that are more tolerant of heat stress.
It isn't clear whether corals in

Hawaii are individually acclimatising
to rising temperatures, or whether
they have genetically evolved a heat
tolerance that they will pass onto
future generations, says Mikhail Matz
at the University of Texas at Austin.
Either way, it is good news, he says,
although genetic adaptation may
have more lasting benefits.
Coles doesn’t think the change will
be enough to counteract the effects
of global warming on coral. But it
suggests that, if we take action on
climate change now, some coral reefs
may still exist in a hundred years’
time, he says. Katarina Zimmer ■

Tracking marbles can test the
ability to reason what others think–
Free download pdf