Clinical Psychology

(Kiana) #1

The Mask of Sanity(Cleckley, 1964) provided
detailed accounts of the lives of psychopaths.
Cases in Behavior Modification(Ullman &
Krasner, 1965) demonstrated the efficacy
of behavioral treatments with single cases.
Nothing will ever likely supplant the case
study as a way of helping clinicians to understand
that unique patient who sits there before them.
As Allport (1961) so compellingly argued, indivi-
duals must be studied individually. Case studies
have been especially useful for (a) providing
descriptions of rare or unusual phenomena or
novel, distinctive methods of interviewing,


assessing, or treating patients; (b) disconfirming
“universally”known or accepted information; and
(c) generating testable hypotheses (Kazdin, 2003)
(see Box 4-2).
There is, of course, a downside to case study
methods. For example, it is difficult to use individ-
ual cases to develop universal laws or behavioral
principles that apply to everyone. Likewise, one
case study cannot lead to cause–effect conclusions
because clinicians are not able to control important
variables that have operated in that case. For exam-
ple, one patient may benefit enormously from psy-
chodynamic therapy for reasons that have less to do
with the therapy method than with the personality

BOX4-2 Clinical Applications: Generating Hypotheses from Therapy

Karl was an unmarried veteran referred to a Veterans
Administration outpatient clinic. It did not take long
for the therapist to realize that Karl’s problems were
not of the typical variety. Karl was a bit anxious or, at
times, depressed, but his main problem seemed to be a
near total lack of interpersonal and social skills. He had
no job, and he lived off his small government pension
along with whatever support his mother could provide.
Aside from his mother, with whom he lived, he rarely
interacted with anyone except perhaps to buy cigar-
ettes or get change from a disinterested bus driver. He
certainly had no friends.
Therapy, then, became not an insight-oriented,
uncovering process but a teaching process. The goals
became teaching Karl to find work, enabling him to
attend night school to learn a trade, and inculcating at
least a few basic social skills. The focus was on how to
find a job, keep a job, talk with others, and gain their
interest. Hour after hour was spent on these tasks
during the therapy.
But progress was slow. It was not that Karl failed
to understand or was totally disinterested or even
loath to try out newly learned skills. The difficulty was
that even when Karl attempted a new behavior and
was successful, that success seemed to have little effect
on his subsequent behavior. This was strange indeed.
Psychologists quickly learn that reinforcement
strengthens the likelihood of the reinforced behavior
in the future, given similar conditions. But not so with
Karl. Reinforcement seemed to do little to raise his
expectancies that the behavior would work again.

Karl almost seemed to want to be the singular excep-
tion to a prime rule of learning theory—that rein-
forcement enhances habit strength!
The therapist and his consultants puzzled over this
for months. For example, after applying for a job and
getting it, Karl’s confidence did not increase at all.
Instead, he attributed his success to luck, not to his
own efforts. Several other similar episodes followed.
After much urging by the therapist, Karl asked a
female coworker for a date. She accepted. But again,
Karl merely remarked on his good luck.
Eventually, the therapist decided that perhaps
Karl believed that the occurrence of reinforcement was
outside his personal control. If so, the inability of suc-
cess to increase his confidence began to make sense.
He was not responding in defiance of learning theory.
Instead, the therapist’s conceptualization had been
incomplete. Reinforcement will“stamp in”a behavior,
but only when that behavior is seen as causally related
to the subsequent reinforcement. Karl believed chance
rather than personal skill was operative. And under
chance conditions, reinforcement carries no implica-
tions for the future. Thus, the riddle of Karl’s behavior
seemed to have been solved. Or at the very least, an
important hypothesis had been formulated. In fact, a
great deal of empirical research lay ahead. Only after
several years of empirical research could the utility of
the chance-versus-skill hypothesis be verified. This gen-
eral research field came to be referred to asinternal–
external control(Rotter, 1966), orlocus of control.

RESEARCH METHODS IN CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY 99
Free download pdf