Representing Relationships Between Categories: Semantic Networks • 253
CRITICISM OF THE COLLINS AND QUILLIAN MODEL
Although Collins and Quillian’s model was supported by the results of a
number of experiments, such as their reaction time experiment (Figure 9.14)
and Meyer and Schvaneveldt’s priming experiment, it didn’t take long for
other researchers to call the theory into question. They pointed out that the
theory couldn’t explain the typicality effect, in which reaction times for state-
ments about an object are faster for more typical members of a category than
for less typical members (see page 244; Rips et al., 1973). Thus, the statement
“A canary is a bird” is verifi ed more quickly than “An ostrich is a bird,” but
the model predicts equally fast reaction times because “canary” and “ostrich”
are both one node away from “bird.”
Researchers also questioned the concept of cognitive economy because
of evidence that people may, in fact, store specifi c properties of concepts (like
“has wings” for “canary”) right at the node for that concept (Conrad, 1972).
In addition, Lance Rips and coworkers (1973) obtained sentence verifi cation
results such as the following:
- A pig is a mammal. RT = 1,476 ms
- A pig is an animal. RT = 1,268 ms
“A pig is an animal” is verifi ed more quickly, but
as we can see from the network in ● Figure 9.18,
the Collins and Quillian model predicts that “A
pig is a mammal” should be verifi ed more quickly
because a link leads directly from “pig” to “mam-
mal,” but we need to travel one link past the mam-
mal node to get to “animal.” Sentence verifi cation
results such as these, plus the other criticisms
of the theory, led Collins and Elizabeth Loftus
(1975) to propose a new semantic network model
designed to handle the results that the Collins and
Quillian model couldn’t explain.
THE COLLINS AND LOFTUS MODEL:
PERSONAL EXPERIENCE AFFECTS
NETWORKS
Collins and Loftus (1975) proposed a model that
resulted in networks like the one in ● Figure 9.19,
in which concepts that are more closely related
are connected by shorter lines. For example, the
network in Figure 9.19 indicates that “vehicle” is
connected to “car,” “truck,” and “bus” by short
links (because these are closely related concepts),
but is connected to “fi re engine” and “ambulance”
(which are less typical vehicles than car, truck, or
bus) by longer links. These shorter links predict
faster reaction times for the more typical vehicles.
The Collins and Loftus model abandons the
hierarchical structure used by Collins and Quillian in
favor of a structure based on a person’s experience.
This means that the spacing between various con-
cepts can differ for various people depending on their
experience and knowledge about specifi c concepts.
In addition to proposing experientially based
links between concepts, Collins and Loftus also
● FIGURE 9.18 Semantic network that
shows that “pig” is closer to “mammal” than
to “animal.”
Animal
Salmon Pig
Fish Mammal
Canary
Bird
● FIGURE 9.19 Semantic network proposed by Collins and Loftus (1975).
(Source: A. M. Collins & E. F. Loftus, “A Spreading-Activation Theory of Semantic Processing,”
Psychological Review, 82, 407–428, Fig. 1. Copyright © 1975 with permission from the
American Psychological Association.)
Street
Ambulance
Car
Vehicle
Truck
Bus
Fire
engine
Apples
Pears
Cherries
Sunsets
Sunrises Clouds
Flowers
Violets Roses
Yellow
Green
Orange Fire
House
Red
Copyright 2011 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part.