Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research and Everyday Experience, 3rd Edition

(Tina Meador) #1

294 • CHAPTER 11 Language


How do we understand
individual words, and
how are words combined
to create sentences? (297)

How can we
understand sentences
that have more than one
meaning? (304)

How do we understand
stories? (309)

Does language affect
the way a person
perceives colors? (316)

Some Questions We Will Consider


L


eaving the Chinese restaurant, I opened my fortune cookie and pulled
out a thin slip of paper. On one side it said “Learn Chinese,” followed by a phrase
and its translation; on the other side it said:

Everything you add to the truth
Subtracts from the truth

As I pondered this statement, it occurred to me how amazing language is and how, like
every cognitive capacity we have described so far, it involves knowledge that we bring
to a situation. Understanding the saying I pulled out of my fortune cookie requires, for
example, an understanding of the concept of “truth,” but what makes it interesting is
the question it poses: How can adding something to a true statement make it less true?
One answer to this question is based on the classic “fi sh story.” A fi sherman boasts that
he caught a big fi sh (which may be true) but then adds to the truth by exaggerating the
fi sh’s length (which makes his account less true). If we have the knowledge of the “fi sh
story” idea that “adding to the truth” generally implies adding something false to the
truth, then the initially puzzling statement from my fortune cookie makes perfect sense.
There is no question that knowledge is an integral part of language. For one thing,
we come into the world without it, and have to learn it. Anyone who has observed
infants for any length of time realizes that they understand language before they can
produce it. Then when they begin talking, the process begins with single words, pro-
gresses to short phrases, and culminates in sentences that increase in complexity as the
infant, then child, learns more on the path to adulthood.

What Is Language?


Long before individuals have acquired the knowledge needed to determine the mean-
ing of my fortune cookie, they will have acquired the ability to create sentences and
string them together into paragraphs that express their thoughts in either written or
spoken form. This ability to understand words and then string them together to express
thoughts makes possible the remarkable feat of transmitting thoughts from one person
to another. This ability is captured in the following defi nition of language: a system
of communication using sounds or symbols that enables us to express our feelings,
thoughts, ideas, and experiences.
But this defi nition doesn’t go far enough, because it conceivably could include some
forms of animal communication. Cats “meow” when their food dish is empty; monkeys
have a repertoire of “calls” that stand for things such as “danger” or “greeting”; bees
perform a “waggle dance” at the hive to indicate the location of fl owers. Although there
is some evidence that monkeys may be able to use language in a way similar to humans
(see “If You Want to Know More: Animal Language,” page 321), most animal com-
munication lacks the properties of human language. Let’s expand on our defi nition by
considering some of the properties that make human language unique.

Copyright 2011 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part.
Free download pdf