Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research and Everyday Experience, 3rd Edition

(Tina Meador) #1
424 • References

Rizzolatti, G., & Arbib, M. A. (1998).
Trends in Neurosciences, 21, 188–194.
Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., &
Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and
the recognition of motor actions.
Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131–141.
Rizzolatti, G., Forgassi, L., & Gallese, V.
(2000). Cortical mechanisms subserving
object grasping and action recognition: A
new view on the cortical motor functions.
In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive
neurosciences (pp. 539–552). Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Rizzolatti, G., Forgassi, L., & Gallese, V.
(2006, November). Mirrors in the mind.
Scientific American, pp. 54–63.
Roberson, D., Davies, I., & Davidoff, J.
(2000). Color categories are not univer-
sal: Replications and new evidence
from a stone-age culture. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General,
129 , 369–398.
Robertson, L., Treisman, A., Freidman-
Hill, S., & Grabowecky, M. (1997).
The interaction of spatial and object
pathways: Evidence from Balint’s
syndrome. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 9, 295–317.
Rock, I. (1983). The logic of perception.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Roediger, H. L. (1990). Implicit memory:
Retention without remembering.
American Psychologist, 45, 1043–1056.
Roediger, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006).
Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory
tests improves long-term retention.
Psychological Science, 17, 249–255.
Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B.
(1995). Creating false memories:
Remembering words not presented in
lists. Journal of Experimental Psycho-
logy: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
21 , 803–814.
Rogers, T. B., Kuiper, N. A., & Kirker, W. S.
(1977). Self-reference and the encoding
of personal information. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 35,
677–688.
Rogers, T. T., & McClelland, J. L. (2004).
Semantic cognition: A parallel distrib-
uted processing approach. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Rogers, T. T., & McClelland, J. L. (2008).
Precis of Semantic Cognition, a Parallel
Distributed Processing Approach
(including open peer commentary and
response to commentaries). Behavioral
and Brain Sciences, 31, 689–749.
Rogin, M. P. (1987). Ronald Reagan, the
movie and other episodes in political
demonology. Berkeley: University of
California Press.

Rolls, E. T., & Tovee, M. J. (1995). Sparse-
ness of the neuronal representation of
stimuli in the primate temporal visual
cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73,
713–726.
Rosch, E. H. (1973). On the internal
structure of perceptual and semantic
categories. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cogni-
tive development and the acquisition of
language (pp. 111–144). New York:
Academic Press.
Rosch, E. H. (1975a). Cognitive representa-
tions of semantic categories. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 104,
192–233.
Rosch, E. H. (1975b). The nature of mental
codes for color categories. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception and Performance, 1, 303–322.
Rosch, E. H., & Mervis, C. B. (1975).
Family resemblances: Studies in the
internal structures of categories.
Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573–605.
Rosch, E. H., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D.,
Johnson, D. M., & Boyes-Braem, P.
(1976). Basic objects in natural catego-
ries. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382–439.
Rosch, E. H., Simpson, C., & Miller, R. S.
(1976). Structural bases of typicality
effects. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and
Performance, 2, 491–502.
Rose, D. (1996). Reflections on (or by?)
grandmother cells. Perception, 25, 881.
Rosenbaum, R. S., Köhler, S., Schacter, D. L.,
Moscovitch, M., Westmacott, R.,
Black, S. E., et al. (2005). The case of
K.C.: Contributions of a memory-
impaired person to memory theory.
Neuropsychologia, 43, 989–1021.
Ross, D. F., Ceci, S. J., Dunning, D., &
Toglia, M. P. (1994). Unconscious trans-
ference and mistaken identity: When a
witness misidentifies a familiar but inno-
cent person. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 79, 918–930.
Rowe, J. B., Owen, A. M., Johnsrude, I. S.,
& Passingham, R. E. (2001). Imaging
the mental components of a planning
task. Neuropsychologia, 39, 315–327.
Rubin, D. C. (2005). A basic-systems
approach to autobiographical memory.
Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 14, 79–83.
Rubin, D. C., Rahhal, T. A., & Poon, L. W.
(1998). Things learned in early adult-
hood are remembered best. Memory &
Cognition, 26, 3–19.
Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L.
(1986). Parallel distributed processing:
Explorations in the microstructure of
cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Rundus, D. (1971). Analysis of rehearsal
processes in free recall. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 89, 63–77.

Sachs, J. (1967). Recognition memory for
syntactic and semantic aspects of a
connected discourse. Perception and
Psychophysics, 2, 437–442.
Sacks, O. W. (1985). The man who mistook
his wife for a hat and other clinical tales.
New York: Summit Books.
Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L.
(1999). Statistical learning of tone
sequences by human infants and adults.
Cognition, 70, 27–52.
Samuel, A. G. (1990). Using perceptual-
restoration effects to explore the
architecture of perception. In G. T. M.
Altmann (Ed.), Cognitive models of
speech processing (pp. 295–314).
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Sanfey, A. G., Lowenstein, G., McClure,
S. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2006).
Neuroeconomics: Cross-currents in
research on decision-making. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 10, 106–116.
Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A.,
Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2003).
The neural basis of economic decision
making in the Ultimatum Game. Science,
300 , 1755–1758.
Sara, S. J. (2000). Strengthening the shaky
trace through retrieval. Neuroscience, 1,
212–213.
Sara, S. J., & Hars, B. (2006). In memory of
consolidation. Learning & Memory, 13,
515–521.
Savin, H. B. (1963). Word-frequency effects
and errors in the perception of speech.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of
America, 35, 200–206.
Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory:
History and current status. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory and Cognition, 13, 501–518.
Schacter, D. L. (2001). The seven sins of
memory. New York: Houghton Mifflin.
Schacter, D. L., Norman, K. A., &
Koutstaal, W. (1998). The cognitive
neuroscience of constructive memory.
Annual Review of Psychology, 49,
289–318.
Schacter, D. L., & Slotnick, S. D. (2004).
The cognitive neuroscience of memory
distortion. Neuron, 44 , 149–160.
Scheck, B., Neufeld, P., & Dwyer, J. (2000).
Actual innocence. New York: Doubleday.
Schenkein, J. (1980). A taxonomy for
repeating action sequences in natural
conversation. In B. Butterworth (Ed.),
Language production (Vol. 1, pp. 21–47).
San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

33559_15_reference_p411-428.indd 42433559_15_reference_p411-428.indd 424 13/04/10 5:46 PM13/04/10 5:46 PM


Copyright 2011 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part.
Free download pdf