Evolution, 4th Edition

(Amelia) #1
328 CHAPTER 13

nestlings like their own young, but some host species do recognize parasite eggs
and either eject them or desert the nest and start a new nest and clutch. Many
brood parasites have counteradapted by laying mimetic eggs [65]. Each popula-
tion of the common cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) contains several different genotypes,
which prefer different hosts and lay eggs closely resembling those of their pre-
ferred hosts (FIGURE 13.9B). Nick Davies and Michael Brooke traced the fate of
artificial cuckoo eggs placed in the nests of various host species [16]. Bird species
that are not parasitized by cuckoos tend not to eject cuckoo eggs. But among the
cuckoos’ preferred hosts, those species whose eggs are mimicked by cuckoos reject
artificial eggs more often than those whose eggs are not mimicked. These species
have adapted to brood parasitism by evolving greater discrimination. Moreover,
populations of two host species that reject artificial cuckoo eggs in Britain accept
them in Iceland, where cuckoos are absent. Thus two evolving traits, host discrimi-
nation and cuckoo egg pattern, shape the evolution of this interaction.

Aposematism and mimicry
Diverse animals, such as bees and coral snakes, have evolved warning, or apo-
sematic, coloration: bright colors that signal to a potential predator that they are
distasteful or dangerous. Predators learn to avoid the color pattern, and so both
the predator and the aposematically colored prey benefit. The warning pattern is
subject to positive frequency-dependent selection because individuals that deviate
from the common pattern, which predators have learned, are likely to be attacked
(see Figure 5.24). Thus, a mutation that confers a new aposematic pattern is likely
to be disadvantageous. How new aposematic phenotypes evolve is therefore a
puzzle. They might be caused by genetic drift in places or at times when selection
by predators is relaxed (see p. 130 in Chapter 5) [48].
Mimicry is a form of convergent evolution in which resemblance between dif-
ferent species has evolved because it is advantageous for members of one species
to resemble another. The species, then, do not owe their resemblance to common
ancestry, but in some cases are so similar that experts have to look very carefully to
distinguish them. The most common kind of mimicry is defensive mimicry, which
often is based on the aposematic coloration of other species [49]. Two common
forms of defensive mimicry are named for the naturalists who first recognized
them. In Batesian mimicry, a palatable species (a mimic) resembles an unpalat-
able species (a model). Selection on a mimetic phenotype can depend on both its
density, relative to that of a model species, and the degree of unpalatability of the
model. A predator that can learn is more likely to avoid eating a butterfly that looks
like an unpalatable model if it has had a recent reinforcing experience (e.g., vomit-
ing after eating a butterfly with that pattern). If the predator has recently swal-
lowed a tasty butterfly, however, it will be more, not less, inclined to eat the next
butterfly with the same phenotype. Thus the rarer a palatable Batesian mimic is
relative to an unpalatable model, the more likely predators are to associate its color
pattern with unpalatability, and so the greater the advantage of resembling the
model will be. If a rare new phenotype arises that mimics a different model spe-
cies, it will have higher fitness, and so a mimetic polymorphism can be maintained
by negative frequency-dependent selection, as is seen in the African swallowtail
Papilio dardanus (FIGURE 13.10).
The other major form of defensive mimicry is Müllerian mimicry, in which two
or more unpalatable species are co-mimics (or co-models) and jointly reinforce
aversion learning by predators. This hypothesis was proposed by Fritz Müller in
1879 and has been confirmed by experiments in which the survival of distaste-
ful mimetic butterflies was shown to be higher if they closely matched an abun-
dant co-mimic species (FIGURE 13.11). This form of mimicry causes positive

13_EVOL4E_CH13.indd 328 3/22/17 1:26 PM

Free download pdf