321435_Print.indd

(やまだぃちぅ) #1

Maternal diets prior to pregnancy, during pregnancy, and when breast feeding


can affect the taste preferences of neonates (Trout and Wetzel-Effinger 2012 ),


infants, and young children (Mennella 2014 ). Nutritional biomarkers from amniotic


fluid, breast milk, and formula can be used to assess prenatal and neonatal exposure


to specific dietary metabolites, nutrients, andflavors (Cooke and Fildes 2011 ; Trout


and Wetzel-Effinger 2012 ). Facial expressions and acceptance or rejection behav-


iors signal preferences in infants (Mennella 2014 ; Steiner 1979 ). Cross-culturally,


mothers use complementary foods to introduce and condition newflavor prefer-


ences in infants (Macbeth and Lowry 1997 ; Schwartz et al. 2013 ). Mennella ( 2014 )


presents a comprehensive review of this area including a number of studies


focusing on the early introduction of fruits and vegetables with a goal of estab-


lishing preferences for these foods as a strategy to counter preferences for processed


sweet and salty foods (Mennella and Trabulsi 2012 ).


Intentional/instructional and unintentional, visible and invisible information about


food preferences and individual, ethnic, social class, age, and gender-appropriate


eating styles are communicated by parents, siblings, teachers, peers, print, and elec-


tronic media (Dufour et al. 2012 ; Kittler and Sucher 2008 ; Pliner and Chaiken 1990 ).


Just as small differences in the physical environment (e.g., music tempo, lighting level,


food odors) can have profound effects on consumption patterns and amount eaten


(Wansink 2010 ), so can small changes in the social environment. For example, people
consumed more food in a restaurant when they were eating with other people: about


35% more with one person, 75% more with 4 people, and 95% more with 7 or more


people (Bell and Pliner 2003 ; Wansink2004b, 2010 ). Experimental evidence indicates


that people unconsciously monitor and mimic eating pacesetters (Wansink 2010 ).


Social network analysis has delineated the long-term roles of spouses, siblings, friends,


and neighbors on eating patterns and body weight (Christakis and Fowler 2007 ;


Pachucki et al. 2011 ). Using the large, longitudinal Framingham Heart Study database,


Christakis and Fowler ( 2007 ) found that a person’s chances of becoming obese


increased by 57% if he or she had a friend who became obese, 40% if one’ssibling


became obese and 37% if one’s spouse became obese.


A possible neurological substrate influencing both short-term mimicking of eating


behaviors and the role of social networks and social cognition in body size and weight


are the Von Economo neurons (VENs) (Allman et al. 2011 ; Stimpson et al. 2011 ).


These distinctive neurons are present in the anterior cingulate (ACC) and


fronto-insular (FI) cortex, both areas related to goal-directed behaviors, such as eating.


They are unique to species with both large brains and complex social systems, pos-


sibly representing convergent evolution as an adaptation to rapid transmission of


socially relevant information. For example, VENs are activated by a number of


emotions: embarrassment, resentment, and empathy (Allman et al. 2011 ). Recent


evidence indicates that protein expression in the VENs regulates gut and immune


function, linking visceral feelings and appetite control to self-awareness and social


reciprocity, i.e.,‘having a gut feeling about someone or some situation’(Stimpson


et al. 2011 ). Biomarkers include morphological features of size, location, and density


of VENs and neurophysiological function as monitored using functional magnetic
resonance imaging (fMRI) or positron emission tomography (PET).


208 L.S. Lieberman

Free download pdf