0851996884.pdf

(WallPaper) #1

using precisely the same set of external stim-
uli. Two types of adaptive variation have
been distinguished in the foraging behaviour
of natural enemies: genetically fixed differ-
ences and phenotypic plasticity. In order to
understand erratic behaviour and to be able
to manipulate such variation, biological con-
trol researchers need to know the origins and
width of variation (Chapters 3 and 4).
Foraging behaviour can also be strongly
influenced by the physiological condition of
the natural enemy. Natural enemies face
varying situations in meeting their food,
mating, reproductive and safety require-
ments. The presence of strong chemical,
visual or auditory cues, cues related to the
presence of enemies of the natural enemy
and (temporary) egg depletion can all reduce
or disrupt the response to cues used to find
hosts. For example, hunger may result in
increased foraging for food and decreased
attention to hosts. In that case, the reaction to
food and host cues will be different from
when the natural enemy is well fed
(Chapters 3, 4 and 5).
The sources of intrinsic variation in forag-
ing behaviour (genetic, phenotypic and those
related to the physiological state) are not
mutually exclusive but overlap extensively,
even within a single individual. The even-
tual foraging effectiveness of a natural
enemy is determined by how well the nat-
ural enemy’s net intrinsic condition is
matched with the foraging environment in
which it operates.


Managing variability in behaviour of natural
enemies

In order to be efficient as biological control
agents, natural enemies must be able to
effectively locate and attack a host and stay
in a host-infested area until most hosts are
attacked. Efficiency as a biological control
agent is used here in the anthropocentric
sense (i.e. our purposes for pest control),
which does not necessarily mean efficiency
from a natural-selection viewpoint. Manage-
ment of natural-enemy variation is particu-
larly important when species are
mass-produced in the laboratory, especially


if rearing is done in factitious hosts (Chapter
9). Such laboratory rearings remove natural
enemies from the context of natural selection
and expose them to artificial selection for
traits that are useless in the field (van
Lenteren, 1986a). In addition to the genetic
component, associative learning may lead to
many more changes in behavioural reactions
(Chapters 3 and 4).

Managing genetic qualities
Successful predation or parasitism of a target
host in a confined situation does not guaran-
tee that released individuals will be suitable
for that host under field conditions. When
selecting among strains of natural enemies,
we need to ensure that the traits of the nat-
ural enemies are appropriately matched with
the targeted use situations in the field.
Natural-enemy populations should perform
well on the target crop and under the specific
climate conditions.

Managing phenotypic qualities
Without care, insectary environments lead to
agents with weak or distorted responses. If
we understand the sources and mechanism
of natural-enemy learning, we can, in theory,
provide the appropriate level of experience
to correct such defects before releasing the
natural enemies. Also, prerelease exposure to
important stimuli can help improve the
responses of natural enemies through asso-
ciative learning, leading to reduction in
escape response and increased arrestment in
target areas.

Managing physical and
physiological qualities
Natural enemies should be released in a
physiological state in which they are most
responsive to herbivore or plant stimuli and
will not be hindered in their responses by
deprivations that interfere with host search-
ing. Thus, adult parasitoids should be well
fed (honey or sugar source available in mass
rearing; Chapter 5), have had opportunities
to mate and have had their preoviposition
period before releases are made.

Need for Quality Control of Biocontrol Agents 13
Free download pdf