Rodent Societies: An Ecological & Evolutionary Perspective

(Greg DeLong) #1

alarm call but does not show its own responses until it can
locomote independently. Second, neither process of influ-
ence implies complex mental states or awareness of ju-
veniles’ abilities by adults (cf. Cheney and Seyfarth 1990).
Third, juveniles may be passive observers or recipients of
adult actions; that is, young do not have to actively seek out
adults and copy their behavior. Fourth, “direct” and “indi-
rect” describe processesrather than outcomesof influence,
and do not necessarily differ in the importance they play
in juvenile behavioral development (Mateo and Holmes
1997). These processes of parental influence on juvenile on-
togeny are potentially common among rodent species, par-
ticularly those with extended parental care, as will be dis-
cussed in the next section.


Development of Survival Behaviors


As a detailed example of the intertwining effects of social
and environmental factors on development of adaptive be-
haviors, consider the suite of survival skills exhibited by an
animal. Many programs of research have focused on the
development of antipredator behavior to identify the spe-
cific factors influencing the expression of unfolding traits.
Some investigators have proposed that survival behaviors,
such as alarm calls or predator-avoidance tactics, are pre-
programmed or innate, so that the behaviors emerge in-
dependent of environmental input (Tinbergen 1953; Bolles
1970; Magurran 1990; Curio 1993). Others have argued
that antipredator behaviors are acquired or learned, through
either direct or indirect experiences with predators and con-
specifics (Vitale 1989; Cheney and Seyfarth 1990; Mateo
1996a). Nature-nurture dichotomous thinking still persists
in the current literature despite acknowledgments by many
that behavior is not innate or acquired per se, but in-
stead develops epigenetically, through interactions between
the organism and the series of environments it encounters
throughout its life span (Lehrman 1970; Gottlieb 1976;
Johnston 1987). Recall that with this approach, behaviors
of young animals are not considered impoverished versions
of adult responses, but instead are ontogenetic adaptations,
functional in their current stage of development (Williams
1966; Alberts 1987).
A behavioral system that required each juvenile to learn
independently how to recognize and respond to predators
would consume time and energy and be prone to fatal er-
rors in learning (Darwin 1859; Bolles 1970). Reliance on
experience, whether it be practice or learning, may at first
appear less than optimal, given the vulnerability of young to
predators, but a flexible developmental program might be
beneficial when predator contexts vary among age groups


or among populations, favoring plasticity of species-typical
behaviors (Johnston 1982; Shettleworth 1998). For ex-
ample, animals that are sympatric with predatory snakes
need to develop antisnake behaviors, whereas animals that
do not encounter snakes obviously do not need to add (or
maintain) these behaviors into their antipredator repertoires
(e.g., Coss and Owings 1985; Coss et al. 1993). Such re-
sponses, then, would not necessarily be present upon first
encounter with predators, but would be acquired rapidly
with additional experience.
Ground-dwelling squirrels are vulnerable to both aerial
(e.g., hawks, eagles, gulls) and terrestrial (e.g., coyotes, wea-
sels, martens, venomous snakes) predators. Despite the ob-
vious survival advantage of avoiding capture by a hunting
predator, young need to learn from which animals to flee, to
which warning calls to respond, and in what manner. Bel-
ding’s ground squirrels (Spermophilus beldingi;fig. 17.2)

Ontogeny of Adaptive Behaviors 199

Figure 17.2 Adult female Belding’s ground squirrel with two of her juvenile
offspring. The left juvenile is engaged in a nasal investigation of its mother, smell-
ing her oral glands, which provide information about individual and kin identity.
Photo by J. M. Mateo.
Free download pdf