Consciousness

(Tuis.) #1

for example, he cannot change his perspective so as to be able
to see what is behind objects in front of him. But other factors
result in a heightened, not a lessened, experience of reality.
Because ayahuasca visions are not mediated by the cornea, iris,
and lens, the gradations in acuity that shape the experiences of
normal vision (with resolution highest at the fovea and much
lower at the periphery) are absent: ‘everything in the inner field
of vision seems to be equally sharp, which may contribute to
the “more real than real” feeling that is so frequently reported
in ayahuasca experiences’ (p. 259).


Experienced ayahuasca users travel in this world or other
worlds, according to their traditions, and describe non-or-
dinary ways of seeing. They claim that the gods, demons,
heavens, and hells that they visit are as real as, or even more
real than, the ordinary world of normal vision. They describe
gaining spiritual insights and a deeper understanding of real-
ity and of themselves.


For Luna (2016), the experiences elicited by ayahuasca over-
whelm him with the feeling that much more is going on than
simply the constructions of his own mind. Drinking ayahuasca makes the idea
that consciousness is limited to humans seem ludicrous. ‘The feeling is rather that
consciousness permeates everything, that it might be primordial’ (p. 268), and that
sacred plants are just one way for humans to tap into its various manifestations.


Could this be?


BUT IS IT REAL?


What are we to make of all this? Like Castaneda in his sceptical anthropologist mode,
we may claim that the experiences are ‘all in the mind’: that they are imaginary and
not real. Indeed, it turns out that Castaneda’s books themselves were more works
of fiction than ethnographic records of research. Writer Richard de Mille made a
thorough study of Castaneda’s works and concluded that ‘Marked anachronisms
or logical conflicts in Castaneda’s work must argue that his text is an imaginative
fabrication rather than a factual report’ (1976, p. 197). The results were a mess: ‘The
wisdom of the ages folded into an omelet with the neurosis of the century’ (p. 18).
Yet Castaneda does force us to wonder about the nature of hallucinations.


A character is either ‘real’ or ‘imaginary’? If you think that, hypocrite


lecteur, I can only smile. You do not even think of your own past as


quite real; you dress it up, you gild it or blacken it, censor it, tinker


with it . . . fictionalize it, in a word, and put it away on a shelf – your


book, your romanced autobiography. We are all in flight from the


real reality. That is a basic definition of Homo sapiens.


(John Fowles, The French Lieutenant’s Woman, 1969/2004, p. 97)

Take those luminous eggs and radiating fibres, reminiscent of the haloes of Chris-
tian saints and the auras of the Theosophical tradition. Auras are a good example
of something that is commonly reported, has consistent features, and yet is not


A

B

FIGURE 14.10 • The doorway test for auras. The
psychic claimant stands facing
the edge of an open doorway.
A target person, whose aura
the psychic says he can see
clearly, takes one of two
possible positions; perhaps five
times each, in random order.
At position (a) neither she nor
her aura should be visible; at
position (b) her body is not
visible but her aura should
easily be seen, sticking out
past the side of the doorframe.
On each trial the psychic must
say whether he sees the aura
sticking out or not. There is no
published evidence that anyone
has ever passed the doorway
test, suggesting that whatever
auras are, they are not
physically present in the space
around the body.
Free download pdf