Australian HiFi — May-June 2017

(Martin Jones) #1

16 Australian


FEATURE


W


hen I was a youngster and an
avid reader of Australian Hi-Fi,
nearly every amplifi er review
contained, it seemed, a mini
rant on the presence of a particular button fi tted
to the amp. Now, many years later, I fi nd myself
writing mini rants on what is essentially the
same subject in most of my reviews of home
theatre receivers.
That control was the ‘Loudness’ control.
These days that control is a great deal more
sophisticated, thanks to digital signal process-
ing technology, and goes under various names
depending on the brand, such as Audyssey
Dynamic EQ, Dolby Volume, YPAO Volume and
so on. These do the intended job better than the
‘Loudness’ control and overcome some of the
inevitable issues it had. But that kind of misses
the point: all of them—and the original ‘Loud-
ness’ button as well—were and are fundamen-
tally misconceived because they are based on a
misunderstanding of how our hearing works.
Our hearing, note, not our ears.
It all started in 1933 when the Fletcher-Mun-
son curves were published. The curves came out
of research that indicated that human hearing
was not linear, but that our sense of loudness
varied by frequency. Over the decades further
experimental work has been conducted, so
there are signifi cant variations from the original
Fletcher-Munson curves and the equal loudness
contours specifi ed in ISO 226.2003, but the
details don’t matter too much to us here.
What does matter is what those curves mean.
They show that if you play a 1kHz tone at 60dB
SPL, a 30Hz tone will have to be more than
80dBSPL to seem like it is equally loud. Taking
things to their end points, extreme treble and

bass can evade detection by the human ear at
much higher levels than mid-frequencies. A
healthy young person can hear a 3kHz or 4kHz
tone at as low a level as –8dBSPL. That is, at an
even quieter level than that originally specifi ed
as the threshold of hearing. But to hear 100Hz it
has to be at least 35dBSPL, and to hear 30Hz it
has to be at least 60dBSPL.
Likewise, a healthy young person’s hearing
is 15dB to 20dB less sensitive at 15kHz than it is
at 3kHz.
Our equipment makers seeing those things
think: ‘Ah, we can shape the sound to get ad-
dress that “problem”. We can boost the bass and
treble when the volume control is low so that
balance is “restored”.’ (And yes, those are all
‘scare’ quotes.) That’s what a ‘Loudness’ control
does. It boosts bass and treble. The better ones
would boost them more at lower settings of the
volume knob, and not at all at higher settings.
There was at least one obvious problem with
that. The amplifi er didn’t know how big your
room was, how far away from the speakers you
were sitting, or how effi cient your speakers were.

And it turns out, the varying sensitivity of
the ear depends not only on frequency but on
volume level. At 100dBSPL (on the newer equal
loudness curves) 30Hz at 100dBSPL sounds close
as dammit in level to 1kHz at 100dBSPL. So
not only were ‘Loudness’ controls fundamen-
tally misconceived, as I am arguing here, they
wouldn’t even work to properly address the
‘problem’ for which they were designed, unless
your system happened to sound exactly as loud
as their ‘corrections’ were designed for.
Modern DSP-based ‘correction’ processes (at
least in theory) deal with this, because they are
set up using data gathered by the receiver in
the automatic room calibration process. So the
processor knows how loud the sound is where
you are sitting and can apply the inverse of the
appropriate equal loudness contour.

Misconceptions
But here’s the thing, you and I and everyone
else does not hear low level sound as defi cient
in those frequencies to which our ears are less
sensitive. Our ears are only part of our hearing
mechanism. The signals from them are fed into
an enormously sophisticated organic signal
processor, the human brain. (Not just humans:
other animals have similar mechanisms).
This OSP does astonishing things. By com-
paring subtle phase differences between the left
and right ear signals it can determine the direc-
tion from which the sound is coming (which is
why bass isn’t directional—the wavelength is too
large for the distance between your ears to allow
the accurate determination of the gap between
the crests of the wave).
And, it turns out, our hearing mechanisms
also have a powerful EQ processor.

Avoid The Loudness


Equal loudness curves, such as the one above,
have been modifi ed over the years.
Free download pdf