Sсiеntifiс Аmеricаn Mind – September – October 2019 (Tablet Edition)

(Wang) #1

plating a new place in town to patron-
ize next week—or next year. Creative
professionals, though, engage other
default network subsystems when
imagining more distant scenarios that
can’t be reconstructed by mixing and
matching different memories that
come to mind. Take the example of a
fiction writer. “They’re imagining
somebody else’s point of view in a
landscape that’s not [the writer’s]
direct reality,” says lead author
Meghan Meyer, an assistant profes-
sor of psychological and brain
science at Dartmouth College.
To uncover how creative profes-
sionals so vividly picture distant or
hypothetical realities, Meyer and her
colleagues performed a series of
three experiments. First, they asked
300 randomly selected study
participants to envision what the
planet would be like in 500 years or
a world in which the continents had
never divided or a life lived as an
angry dictator. Participants were also
asked to think of as many ways as
possible to use a pen or to improve a
megaphone. Those who scored high
on creativity were rated as better at
using distal imagination.
Next, the researchers repeated
these tests with 100 participants who


had demonstrated some sort of
expertise in creativity—writers, actors,
directors and visual artists, who had
received awards in their fields. They
also asked a group of equally suc-
cessful finance, legal and medical
professionals the same questions.
The creative professionals outper-
formed others in written responses
and in self-reports of how vividly
they could picture the situations in
their minds.
Meyer and her team wondered
whether the creative professionals
simply had stronger “imagination
muscles,” the way professional
baseball players have more robust
throwing arms compared with
nonathletes. To see these imagina-
tion muscles in action, they asked
27 creative types and 26 control
participants to go through simulation
tasks while lying in a functional
magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)
scanner. Brain activity of the creative
adepts and controls was similar when
imagining the next 24 hours, but to
the researchers’ surprise, the creative
group alone engaged the dorsomedi-
al default network when imagining
events further into the future.
The dorsomedial default network
was not active at all among the

control group. Yet this network was
switched on even when creative
professionals were at rest. “This is a
big step forward in understanding the
creative brain,” says Roger Beaty, a
psychology researcher at Penn State
University, who was not involved in
the study. “The findings provide
insight into how the brain is able to
imagine different situations and what
makes creative experts exceptional at
imagining distant ones,” he says.
The results also have implications
for the way we consider other people.
Because the dorsomedial default net -
work is involved in thinking about per -
spectives that differ significantly from
our own experiences, people who are
able to activate this network may be
better able to empathize with others
or imagine how public policies may
impact future gene rations, says Dan -
iel Schacter, a Harvard psychologist,
who was not involved in the study.
The next big question is whether
activation of the dorsomedial default
network can be improved with
training, Schacter says. If it is a
malleable ability, maybe taking
drawing classes or the like will boost
our imagination and help us all better
connect with others.
—Knvul Sheikh

Better Memory
through Electrical
Brain Ripples
A study in mice shows improved
cognitive performance when these
bursting signals move around
memory circuits

SPECIFIC PATTERNS OF BRAIN
activity are thought to underlie
specific processes or computations
important for various mental facul-
ties, such as memory. One such
“brain signal” that has received
a lot of attention recently is known
as a sharp wave ripple—a short,
wave-shaped burst of high-frequen-
cy oscillations.
Researchers originally identified
ripples in the hippocampus, a region
crucially involved in memory and
navigation, as central to diverting
recollections to long-term memory
during sleep. Then, a 2012 study by
neuroscientists at the University of
California, San Francisco, led by
Loren Frank and Shantanu Jadhav,
the latter now at Brandeis University,
showed that the ripples also play a
role in memory while awake. The

NEWS

Free download pdf