177
Our view of ourselves, of who we
are, is developed from birth through
interaction with those closest to us.
Individual selves are not the products
of biology but rather of this interaction.
See also: W.E.B. Du Bois 68–73 ■ Edward Said 80–81 ■ Norbert Elias 180–81 ■ Erving Goffman 190–95 ■ Stuart Hall
200–01 ■ Benedict Anderson 202–03 ■ Howard S. Becker 280–85 ■ Adrienne Rich 304–09 ■ Jeffrey Weeks 324–25
CULTURE AND IDENTITY
G
eorge Herbert Mead was a
social psychologist and a
philosopher, and he looked
to both disciplines in trying to work
out what exactly we mean when
we talk about the “self.” Traditional
philosophers and sociologists saw
societies as growing from the
coming together of individual,
autonomous selves, but Mead said
the opposite was true—selves
emerge from social interactions;
they are formed within society.
This concept is prevalent now
in psychology and psychotherapy,
but when Mead first presented
his ideas in 1913 in The Social Self,
it was a revolutionary point of view.
Mead disagreed with the idea
that individual, experiencing selves
exist in any recognizable way
before they are part of the social
process. The social process
of experience or behavior is
“logically prior to the individuals
and their individual experiencing
which are involved in it.”
By this, Mead is suggesting that
an individual’s consciousness, with
all its intentions, desires, and so on,
is formed within the context of
social relationships, one or more
particular languages, and a set of
cultural norms. From birth, babies
begin to sense communication
through gestures, which function
as symbols and build “a universe of
discourse.” Over time, they learn to
mimic and “import” the practices,
gestures, and eventually words of
those around them, so that they
can make their own response and
receive further gestures and words
from others.
Who we are
The pattern of attitudes that the
baby experiences and internalizes
(learns) creates the sense of “me.”
In this way, the “me” represents
the behaviors, expectations,
and attitudes learned through
interactions with others.
But Mead says that we also
have another sense of ourselves,
which he calls the “I.” Both the “I”
and the “me” are different functions
of the self. The “I,” like the “me,”
keeps evolving, but its function is
to reflect on the “me,” while also
seeing the bigger picture: the “me”
acts in habitual ways, while the “I”
can reflect on these and make self-
conscious choices. It allows us
to be different, both from other
people and our former selves,
through reflection on our actions.
Mead’s theory of the
development of self was pivotal
in turning psychology and
sociology away from the idea
of “self” as being merely internal
introspection, and aligning it
firmly within a societal context. ■
G.H. Mead
George Herbert Mead was
born in Massachusetts. His
father was a minister in the
Congregational Church, and
he moved the family to Oberlin,
Ohio, to teach at the seminary
there when Mead was six years
old. After graduating from
Oberlin College in 1883, Mead
worked for a few years as a
teacher and then as a railroad
surveyor before returning to
academia. He began his studies
in philosophy and sociology at
Harvard University in 1887 and
seven years later moved to the
University of Chicago, where he
worked until his death in 1931.
He claimed to have an “activist
spirit” and marched in support
of women’s suffrage and other
causes. The philosopher John
Dewey acknowledged Mead as
having “a seminal mind of the
very first order.”
Key works
1913 The Social Self
1932 The Philosophy of the
Present
1934 Mind, Self, and Society
Mind can never
find expression, and
could never have come
into existence at all,
except in terms of
a social environment.
G.H. Mead