Invitation to Psychology

(Barry) #1

308 Chapter 9 Learning and Conditioning


mouth watering. These new salivary responses
clearly were not inborn, so they must have been
acquired through experience.
At first, Pavlov treated the dog’s drooling as
just an annoying secretion. But he quickly realized
that his assistant had stumbled onto an important
phenomenon, one that Pavlov came to believe
was the basis of most learning in human beings
and other animals (Pavlov, 1927). He called that
phenomenon a “conditional” reflex because it de-
pended on environmental conditions. Later, an er-
ror in the translation of his writings transformed
“conditional” into “conditioned,” the word most
commonly used today.
Pavlov soon dropped what he had been doing
and turned to the study of conditioned reflexes, to
which he devoted the last three decades of his life.
Why were his dogs salivating to things other than
food?

New Reflexes from Old LO 9.1
Pavlov initially speculated about what his dogs
might be thinking and feeling when they drooled
before getting their food. Was the doggy equiva-
lent of “Oh boy, this means chow time” going
through their minds? He soon decided, however,
that such speculation was pointless. Instead, he fo-
cused on analyzing the environment in which the
conditioned reflex arose.
The original salivary reflex, according to
Pavlov, consisted of an unconditioned stimulus (US),
food in the dog’s mouth, and an unconditioned
response (UR), salivation. By an unconditioned

unconditioned stimu-
lus (US) The classical-
conditioning term for
a stimulus that already
elicits a certain response
without additional
learning.


unconditioned
response (UR) The
classical-conditioning
term for a response elic-
ited by an unconditioned
stimulus.


You are about to learn...
• how classical conditioning explains why a dog
might salivate when it sees a light bulb or hears
a buzzer.
• four important features of classical conditioning.
• what is actually learned in classical conditioning.

Classical Conditioning
At the turn of the twentieth century, the great
Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov (1849–1936) was
studying salivation in dogs as part of a research
program on digestion. One of his procedures was
to make a surgical opening in a dog’s cheek and
insert a tube that conducted saliva away from the
animal’s salivary gland so that the saliva could be
measured. To stimulate the reflexive flow of saliva,
Pavlov placed meat powder or other food in the
dog’s mouth (see Figure 9.1).
Pavlov was a truly dedicated scientific ob-
server. Many years later, as he lay dying, he even
dictated his sensations for posterity! And he
instilled in his students and assistants the same
passion for detail. During his salivation stud-
ies, one of the assistants noticed something that
most people would have overlooked or dismissed
as trivial. After a dog had been brought to the
laboratory a few times, it would start to salivate
before the food was placed in its mouth. The sight
or smell of the food, the dish in which the food
was kept, and even the sight of the person who
delivered the food were enough to start the dog’s

FigURE 9.1 Pavlov’s Method
The photo shows Ivan Pavlov (in the white beard), flanked by his students and a canine subject. The drawing depicts
an apparatus similar to the one he used; saliva from a dog’s cheek flowed down a tube and was measured by the
movement of a needle on a revolving drum.
Free download pdf