Invitation to Psychology

(Barry) #1

364 ChapteR 10 Behavior in Social and Cultural Context


grades is being selfish and disrespectful of adults.
But Australian students, who value individualism,
think that the same behavior is perfectly appropri-
ate (Forgas & Bond, 1985). You can see how the
Chinese might form negative stereotypes of “dis-
respectful” Australians, and how the Australians
might form negative stereotypes of the “spineless”
Chinese. And it is a small step from negative ste-
reotypes to prejudice.
Watch the Video In the Real World: Are Stereo-
types and Prejudice Inevitable? at MyPsychLab

of feminists and fraternity men. Stereotypes can be
positive (“Scots are thrifty and smart with money”)
or negative (“Scots are miserly and stingy”).
Moreover, some stereotypes are statistically
accurate; soccer moms are more likely than single
professionals to drive SUVs, after all (Jussim et al.,
2009). Stereotypes therefore are, as some psychol-
ogists have called them, useful tools in the mental
toolbox—energy-saving devices that allow us to
make efficient decisions (Macrae & Bodenhausen,
2000). They help us quickly process new infor-
mation and retrieve memories. They allow us to
organize experience, make sense of differences
among individuals and groups, and predict how
people will behave. In fact, the brain automati-
cally registers and encodes the basic categories of
gender, ethnicity, and age, suggesting that there is
a neurological basis for the cognitive efficiency of
stereotyping (Ito & Urland, 2003).
However, although stereotypes may reflect
real differences among people, they also distort
that reality in three ways (Judd et al., 1995). First,
they exaggerate differences between groups, making
the stereotyped group seem odd, unfamiliar, or
dangerous, not like “us.” Second, they produce selec-
tive perception; people tend to see only the evidence
that fits the stereotype and reject any perceptions
that do not fit. Third, they underestimate differences
within the stereotyped group, creating the impression
that all members of that group are the same.
Cultural values affect how people evaluate the
actions of another group and whether a stereotype
becomes positive or negative. Chinese students
in Hong Kong, where communalism and respect
for elders are valued, think that a student who
comes late to class or argues with a parent about

What is this woman’s occupation? Among non-Muslims
in the West, the assumption is that Muslim women who
wear the full-length black niqab must be repressed sexu-
ally as well as politically. But the answer shatters the ste-
reotype. Wedad Lootah, a Muslim living in Dubai, United
Arab Emirates, is a marriage counselor and sexual activ-
ist, and author of a best-selling Arabic book, Top Secret:
Sexual Guidance for Married Couples. She wrote it, she
says, because of a 52-year-old client who had many chil-
dren but had never experienced sexual pleasure with her
husband. “Finally, she discovered orgasm!” Ms. Lootah
reported. “Imagine, all that time she did not know.”

Percentage of boys who had abest friend in the out-group

40

30

20

10

0
After cooperative
activities were introduced

Before cooperative
activities were introduced

Eagles Rattlers

Figure 10.4 The experiment at robbers Cave
In this study, competitive games fostered hostility between the Rattlers and the Eagles. Few boys had a best friend
from the other group (left). But after the teams had to cooperate to solve various problems, the percentage who made
friends across “enemy lines” shot up (right) (Sherif et al., 1961).
Free download pdf