Persuasive Communication - How Audiences Decide. 2nd Edition

(Marvins-Underground-K-12) #1
Heuristics and Biases in Audience Decision Making 239

recommend to a younger brother or sister. They were also asked to rate the importance of all the


attributes or decision criteria mentioned in the paragraphs. When making their decisions, the


students discounted the slot values of the unique attributes despite claiming they believed that


information to be important.^377


When a format makes it too diffi cult for audiences to compare alternatives along a common

dimension or decision criterion, they may discount that dimension regardless of its importance


to them. An unusual format, recommended by National Academy of Sciences in 1996, displayed


automobile safety information in a way that made it hard for consumers to compare the safety levels


of new vehicles. As a result, consumers did not give safety data the weight it deserved. Instead, con-


sumers gave undue weight to less relevant but easier-to-compare information such as a car’s color


and style. Consumers make better decisions when displays of safety information use meaningful


symbols such as letter grades or stars that make comparing safety ratings easy.^378


Some consumers have developed strategies for avoiding the common dimension effect. When

making choices between comparable products (e.g., two toasters), consumers can easily compare


slot values for the concrete attributes of the products, for example, the number of settings on each


toaster. But some products have few concrete attributes in common (e.g., a toaster and a smoke


detector). To choose among noncomparable products such as these, many consumers have learned


to rise to a higher level of abstraction where they can identify different slot values on common


dimensions.^379 For example, consumers might compare their need for a toaster versus their need for


a smoke detector.


The Asymmetric Dominance Effect: The Impact of a Third Option


Audiences fi nd it easier to make trade-offs between two equally desirable alternatives if given a


third alternative whose slot values are clearly inferior to one alternative but not to the other. The


presence of a third alternative gives audiences a compelling reason to choose one alternative over


the other and is known as the asymmetric dominance effect.^380 For example, when students were given


a choice between $6.00 in cash and a nice pen, 64% chose the money and 36% chose the pen.^381


But when students were given a choice between $6.00, a nice pen, and a cheap pen, only 52%


chose the money. Forty-six percent chose the nice pen and the remaining 2% chose the cheap


pen. Because comparing two pens is easier than comparing a pen to cash, an additional 10% of the


students chose the nice pen.


The Limited Options Bias: The Appeal of Yes or No Choices


Providing audiences with too many alternatives, and thus with too many schema slot values to


integrate easily, can stop them from making a decision. For example, employees’ participation in


401(k) plans dramatically drops as the number of funds their employer allows them to choose


from increases.^382 Experienced physicians are less likely to prescribe a new medication if they


are asked to choose between two medications than if they are asked whether to prescribe a single


medication.^383


In an often-cited study of consumer decision making, grocery store shoppers were offered the

opportunity to taste any of six jams at one booth, or any of 24 jams at another. At the six-jam


booth, 40% of shoppers stopped to taste and 30% who tasted purchased a jam. At the 24-jam booth,


60% stopped to taste but only 3% who tasted purchased.^384 Similar effects have been shown for


other types of products,^385 for loan offers,^386 and for retirement plans.^387 When, under large choice


set conditions, the audience does make a decision, it tends to select predefi ned default options.^388


The audience also exhibits more disappointment and regret.^389

Free download pdf