92 CHAPTER 4 | Primate Behavior
zoo of Madrid, Spain. It began
when a 5-year-old female
rubbed apples against a
sharp corner of a concrete
wall in order to lick the
mashed pieces and juice
left on the wall. From this
youngster, the practice
of “smearing” spread to
her peers, and within five
years most group mem-
bers were performing the
operation frequently and
consistently. The innova-
tion has become standard-
ized and durable, having
transcended two generations in the group.^22
Another dramatic example of learning is afforded by
the way chimpanzees in West Africa crack open oil-palm
nuts. For this they use tools: an anvil stone with a level
surface on which to place the nut and a good-sized ham-
mer stone to crack it. Not any stone will do; it must be of
the right shape and weight, and the anvil may require lev-
eling by placing smaller stones beneath one or more edges.
Nor does random banging away do the job; the nut has to
be hit at the right speed and the right trajectory, or else the
nut simply flies off into the forest. Last but not least, the
apes must avoid mashing their fingers, rather than the nut.
According to fieldworkers, the expertise of the chimps far
exceeds that of any human who tries cracking these hard-
est nuts in the world.
Youngsters learn this process by staying near to adults
who are cracking nuts, where their mothers share some
of the food. This teaches them about the edibility of the
nuts but not how to get at what is edible. This they learn
by observing and by “aping” (copying) the adults. At first
they play with a nut or stone alone; later they begin to ran-
domly combine objects. They soon learn, however, that
placing nuts on anvils and hitting them with a hand or
foot gets them nowhere.
Only after three years of futile effort do they begin to
coordinate all of the multiple actions and objects, but even
then it is only after a great deal of practice, by the age of
6 or 7 years, that they become proficient in this task. They
do this for over a thousand days. Evidently, it is social mo-
tivation that accounts for their perseverance after at least
three years of failure, with no reward to reinforce their ef-
fort. At first, they are motivated by a desire to act like the
In the 1950s and early 1960s, one particularly bright
young female macaque named Imo (Japanese primatolo-
gists always considered it appropriate to name individual
animals) started several innovative behaviors in her troop.
She figured out that grain could be separated from sand if
it was placed in water. The sand sank and the grain floated
clean, making it much easier to eat. She also began the
practice of washing the sweet potatoes that primatologists
provided—first in fresh water but later in the ocean, pre-
sumably because of the pleasant taste the saltwater added.
In each case, these innovations were initially imitated by
other young animals; Imo’s mother was the lone older ma-
caque to embrace the innovations right away.
One newly discovered example is a technique of food
manipulation on the part of captive chimpanzees in the
In the same way that young Imo got her troop to begin washing
sweet potatoes in saltwater, at Kyoto University’s Koshima Island
Primatology Research Preserve another young female macaque
recently taught other macaques to bathe in hot springs. In the
Nagano Mountains of Japan, this macaque, named Mukbili, began
bathing in the springs. Others followed her, and now this is an
activity practiced by all members of the group.
© age fotostock/SuperStock
Pacific
Ocean
Philippine
Sea
East
China
Sea
Sea of
Japan
Nagano
Tokyo
JAPAN
CHINA RUSSIA
NORTH
VIETNAM
SOUTH
VIETNAM
Koshima
(^22) Fernandez-Carriba, S., & Loeches, A. (2001). Fruit smearing by captive
chimpanzees: A newly observed food-processing behavior. Current Anthro-
pology 42, 143–147.