Child Development

(Frankie) #1

Burman, Erica. Deconstructing Developmental Psychology. London:
Routledge, 1994.
Burman, Erica. ‘‘Continuities and Discontinuities in Interpretive
and Textual Approaches in Developmental Psychology.’’
Human Development 39 (1996):330–345.
Chandler, Michael. ‘‘Stumping for Progress in a Post-Modern
World.’’ In E. Amsel and K. A. Renninger eds., Change and De-
velopment: Issues of Method, Theory, and Application. Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1997.
Cole, Michael. ‘‘Cross-Cultural Research in the Sociohistorical Tra-
dition.’’ Human Development 31 (1988):137–157.
Erikson, Erik. Childhood and Society. New York: Norton, 1950.
Erikson, Erik. Identity: Youth and Crisis. London: Faber, 1968.
Erikson, Erik. Identity and the Life Cycle. New York: Norton, 1980.
Erikson, Erik. The Life Cycle Completed. New York: Norton, 1997.
Freud, Sigmund. The Interpretation of Dreams. New York: Modern
Library, 1900.
Freud, Sigmund. The Ego and the Id. London: Hogarth Press, 1927.
Gergen, Kenneth. ‘‘Stability, Change, and Chance in Understand-
ing Human Development.’’ In N. Datan and H. W. Reese eds.,
Life-Span Development Psychology. New York: Academic Press,
1977.
Gilligan, Carol. In a Different Voice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni-
versity Press, 1982.
Goldhaber, Dale. Theories of Human Development: An Integrative Ap-
proach. Mountain View, CA: Mayfield, 2000.
Overton, Willis. ‘‘The Structure of Developmental Theory.’’ In H.
W. Reese ed., Advances in Child Development and Behavior. San
Diego: Academic Press, 1991.
Patterson, Gerald. ‘‘Maternal Rejection: Determinant or Product
for Deviant Child Behavior?’’ In W. W. Hartup and Z. Rubin
eds., Relationships and Development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum, 1986.
Pepper, Stephen. World Hypotheses: A Study in Evidence. Los Ange-
les: University of California Press, 1961.
Plomin, Robert. Development, Genetics and Psychology. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum, 1986.
Plomin, Robert. Genetics and Experience: The Interplay between Nature
and Nurture. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1994.
Reese, Hayne, and W. F. Overton. ‘‘Models of Development and
Theories of Development.’’ In L. R. Goulet and P. B. Baltes
eds., Life-Span Developmental Psychology. New York: Academic
Press, 1970.
Rogoff, Barbara. Apprenticeship in Thinking: Cognitive Development in
Social Context. New York: Oxford University Press, 1990.
Scarr, Sandra, and K. McCartney. ‘‘How People Make Their Own
Environments: A Theory of Genotype-Environment Effects.’’
Child Development 54 (1983):424–435.
Scarr, Sandra, and A. Ricciuti. ‘‘What Effects Do Parents Have on
Their Children?’’ In L. Okagaki and R. J. Sternberg eds., Di-
rectors of Development: Influences on the Development of Children’s
Thinking. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1991.
Skinner, Burris F. The Behavior of Organisms: An Experimental Analy-
sis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1938.
Skinner, Burris F. Walden Two. New York: Macmillan, 1948.
Skinner, Burris F. Science and Human Behavior. New York: Free
Press, 1953.
Vygotsky, Lev. Thought and Language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press,
1962.
Vygotsky, Lev. Mind in Society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press, 1978.
Dale E. Goldhaber


THEORY OF MIND
Theory of mind (ToM) is the understanding of the
mental states of others, including their intentions, de-
sires, beliefs, and emotions. Around the age of eigh-
teen months, a child is able to understand the
intentions of other people. This progresses until the
ages of three to four-and-a-half when children begin
to understand others’ states of knowledge and belief.
Janet Astington, a long-time ToM researcher, has
found that children’s performance on standard false
belief tasks was associated with their production of
joint proposals and explicit role assignments during
pretend play. Success at joint play may have some de-
pendence on representing differing and conflicting
beliefs and goals with the other participant in the play
session. For social development, it is necessary for
children to be able to communicate this information
amongst themselves. With no knowledge about the
mind or their playmate’s beliefs, these pretend play
interactions are quite difficult to execute successfully.

See also: COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT; EMOTIONAL
DEVELOPMENT

Bibliography
Astington, Janet W. ‘‘Intention in the Child’s Theory of Mind.’’ In
Douglas Frye and Chris Moore eds., Children’s Theories of
Mind: Mental States and Social Understanding. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum, 1991.
Astington, Janet W. Child’s Discovery of the Mind. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press, 1993.
Astington, Janet W., Paul L. Harris, and David R. Olson, eds. Devel-
oping Theories of Mind. Cambridge, Eng.: Cambridge Universi-
ty Press, 1990.
Frye, Douglas, and Chris Moore, eds. Children’s Theories of Mind:
Mental States and Social Understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum, 1991.
Harris, Paul L., Carl N. Johnson, Deborah Hutton, and Giles An-
drews. ‘‘Young Children’s Theory of Mind and Emotion.’’
Cognition and Emotion, 3, no. 4 (1989):379–400.
Heyes, Cecilia M. ‘‘Theory of Mind in Nonhuman Primates.’’ Be-
havioral and Brain Sciences 21, no. 1 (1998):101–134.
Perner, Josef, Ted Rufman, and Susan R. Leekam. ‘‘Theory of
Mind Is Contagious: You Catch It from Your Sibs.’’ Child De-
velopment 65 (1994):1228–1238.
Sara Salkind

THREE MOUNTAIN TASK
The Three Mountain Task was developed by Jean
Piaget and Bärbel Inhelder in the 1940s to study chil-
dren’s ability to coordinate spatial perspectives. In
the task, a child faced a display of three model moun-
tains while a researcher placed a doll at different view-
points of the display. The researcher asked the child
to reconstruct the display from the doll’s perspective,
select from a set of pictures showing the doll’s view,

THREE MOUNTAIN TASK 417
Free download pdf