curriculum. What we need in education is the co-ordination of the two’ (Athey, 2002,
p. 10).
Skemp explains the functions of schemas by saying that they integrate existing
knowledge, act as a tool for future learning and that they make understanding pos-
sible. Skemp recognises the importance of schematic development in understanding
mathematics. Like most mathematicians, Skemp’s analysis of schemas is from a
mathematical viewpoint: these are the ‘logico-mathematical’ concepts that children
need to learn first before they can understand higher concepts. As he reflects on
teaching young children, Skemp acknowledges that they already know some
number concepts before they start in an educational setting. He asks if it matters that
they do not yet have an understanding of sorting, sets and matching and one-to-one
correspondence, as long as they ‘tag on’ these concepts at some point. Skemp seems
to wrestle with young children’s schemas because his ideas somehow do not totally
fit into his current knowledge of young children’s development. He has followed the
Piagetian theory and the research model of clinical tasks and has not balanced this
by following the young child in real-life situations; at home, at play or in
autonomous situations where they are following their own thinking (Skemp, 1971).
Figure 3.1 Note from Chloë’s mother
Piaget particularly looked at children’s very early schematic behaviour such as a
baby dropping an object on the floor and the parent retrieving it, only to find five
seconds later that the baby has dropped the object again. This Piaget called ‘object
permanence’ (Piaget, 1958): the child may be thinking ‘is the object still there if I
Mathematical schemas 37
8657part 1b.qxd 04/07/2006 18:07 Page 37