Figure 5.1 Scott’s addition
Understanding symbols
Semiotics
Any serious discussion concerning the development of symbols concerns semiotic
activity which, concerning children’s representations, Oers defines as: ‘the activity of
relating a sign and its meaning, including use of signs, the activity of investigating the
relationship (changes of) signs and (changes of) meaning, as well as improving the
existing relationship between sign (or sign system) and meaning (and meaning
system)’ (Oers, 1997, p. 239). This is what White termed the symbolic initiative (1949).
Gardner views our complex use of symbols as ‘our final building block’. He argues that
‘the disciplines of our world, reconstructed on the basis of symbols; and our capacity
to master them, and to invent new systems, also presupposes the symbolic fluency that
is launched in the years after infancy’ (Gardner, 1997, p. 21).
From a Vygotskian perspective, symbols or graphic representations bridge the gap
between ‘enactive, perception-bound thinking and abstract, symbolical thinking’
(Oers, 1997, p. 237).
Understanding abstract mathematical symbols begins long before children enter
school, with a ‘pre-history’ that Vygotsky believed originates in both gesture and alter-
native meanings that children assign to objects within their play. An example of this is
Melanie’s ladybird (Figure 6.1) in Chapter 6. DeLoache observed that children as young
as 18 months old can pretend that ‘a block of wood is a car, or that a banana is a tele-
phone’: in doing this they demonstrate that they are able to represent something in two
different ways (DeLoache, 1991, p. 749). This flexibility of meaning and object allows
children later to understand that marks – or written symbols on a page – stand for
72 Children’s Mathematics
8657part 1b.qxd 04/07/2006 18:11 Page 72