BIRD BRAINIACS 115``````Recognizing Faces,QZLOGOLIHELRORJLVW-RKQ0DU]OXÆDQGRQHRIKLVVWXGHQWVDWWKH8QLYHUVLW\RI:DVKLQJWRQLQ6HDWWOHGRQQHGPDVNVOLNHWKLVDQGFDSWXUHGDQGEDQGHGVHYHQFURZV7RGD\LI0DU]OXÆRUDQRWKHUSHUVRQSXWVRQWKHPDVNWKHFURZVLQWKHDUHDÅQRWMXVWWKHRULJLQDOVHYHQÅJDWKHUWRVFROGGLYHERPEDQGIROORZKLP7KHFURZVGRQÅ WSHVWHUSHRSOHZHDULQJRWKHUPDVNV
with an uncanny ability to distinguish humanfaces and expressions, letters of the alphabet,even paintings by Monet and Picasso. Other re-searchers revealed the remarkable memories ofClarkâs nutcrackers, scrub jays, and chickadees.Nutcrackers, for example, harvest and cachemore than 30,000 pine seeds every autumn, dis-tributing them in several thousand tiny cachesthey need to remember through the winter.IN THE 1950S, researchers began studying howsongbirds such as canaries, sparrows, and zebrafinches learn their songs. They found strikingsimilarities between birdsong and human speech.And then there was Alex, an African gray parrotwhom comparative psychologist Irene Pepperbergtaught to reproduce English sounds (Pepper-berg emphasizes that Alex did not actually learnthe English language). By the time of his death in2007 at age 31, Alex had mastered roughly a hun-dred English sounds for colors, objects, numbers,and shapes. He could clearly pronounce âgreen,ââyellow,â âwool,â âwood,â âwalnut,â and âbanana,âand used these sounds to communicate with peo-ple. He understood âsameâ and âdifferent,â couldcount to eight, and grasped the abstract conceptof zero, or ânone,â as he called it. Alex used histalent to talk back, telling Pepperberg to âcalmdownâ when she was in a bad mood, and askingto âgo backâ when he yearned for his home duringan illness that kept him at the vetâs. And he alwayswished her a good night, as he did just before hedied. âYou be good. See you tomorrow. I love you.âYet because scientists misunderstood the avianbrain, most of Pepperbergâs discoveries about Alexwere ignored or mocked; she feared that other re-searchers thought she was somehow giving Alexclues. âHis brain was the size of a shelled walnut,âPepperberg concedes, âso some people thoughtI was making it all up or that he was cheating.â(Pepperberg videotaped many experiments withAlex.) Of course, a cheating bird would also be anintelligent bird, but the upshot was simply thatfew scientists in cognition research recognizedAlexâs achievementsâuntil a couple of yearsbefore he died. Alexâs ability to imitate languageâand apparently to use English words in the correctcontextâhas spurred studies of parrots to betterunderstand the origins of vocal learning, which isthe ability to imitate purposeful sounds. This is askill parrots share with songbirds, hummingbirds,humans, cetaceans, and a few other species.The discoveries finally led an internation-al team of researchers to reexamine the long-standing Edinger model of avian neural anatomy.In 2005 they issued their reevaluation, revealingthat birdsâ brains do possess neural structures,called the pallium, that resemble the mammali-an neocortex and other areas associated with so-phisticated thinking. They supported adopting anew nomenclature and understanding of birdsâneural anatomy.âThe parts of birdsâ and mammalsâ brains arearranged differently,â explains Nicola Clayton, apsychologist at the University of Cambridge, inthe United Kingdom, who studies corvid cogni-tion. âMammalsâ brains are layered like a clubsandwich, while birdsâ brains are more like a
martin jones
(Martin Jones)
#1