Abnormal Psychology

(やまだぃちぅ) #1

Clinical Diagnosis and Assessment 97


years with the Air Force; his stories often involved his heroic actions that saved others


(such as fi xing a broken sluicegate at Hoover Dam or safely landing a plane after an


engine failed). According to Jeannette, “Dad always fought harder, fl ew faster, and


gambled smarter than everyone else in his stories. Along the way, he rescued women


and children and even men who weren’t as strong and clever” (Walls, 2005, p. 24). A


clinical assessment could help determine the extent to which Rex’s stories were merely


embellishments to make him look good or were delusions. It can be useful to obtain


information from family, friends, or other people who are in a position to confi rm or


modify a patient’s report in order to distinguish delusions from embellishments.


Here’s an example of how simply asking patients about themselves during clini-

cal interviews may not always lead to an accurate picture of their psychological


functioning, despite their honesty. One of the authors of this book (Rosenberg)


once worked in a hospital emergency room, evaluating people who came to the


emergency room for psychiatric reasons. One day, she was asked to interview a


man in his 30s who was dressed appropriately and spoke somewhat slowly. He had


a history of schizophrenia and depression, and his psychiatrist was affi liated with


the hospital. The patient reported that he came to the hospital because his mother


wanted him to come (in fact, she drove him there), but he wasn’t exactly sure why.


He said that he wasn’t hearing voices, and he didn’t feel sad, depressed, or suicidal.


In response to questions, his mental processes and mental contents seemed normal.


Rosenberg telephoned his psychiatrist, who was puzzled about why the patient had


come to the emergency room. The psychiatrist could shed no light on the matter; he


had an appointment scheduled with the patient the following week, but he did not


know why the patient’s mother brought him to the hospital.


Rosenberg asked the patient’s permission to speak with his mother; he consented.

The mother, a woman in her 60s, reported that she thought her son had been more


depressed lately. She went on to explain that for the last month he hadn’t gotten out


of bed unless he had a doctor’s appointment. On those days, she harangued him until


he bathed, groomed himself, and put on appropriate clothes (as she had also done


the day he came to the emergency room). The other days he stayed in bed in his pa-


jamas. When Rosenberg spoke to the psychiatrist, he had not known that his patient


was depressed, or that the only reason the patient was appropriately dressed and


groomed for appointments was because of his mother’s insistence. When Rosenberg


then spoke with the patient again, explaining what the mother had said, he admitted


that he had a hard time getting out of bed, and didn’t have much energy or interest in


things, although he didn’t feel depressed. He agreed that it made sense for him (and


his mother) to be interviewed by staff in the inpatient psychiatric unit to determine


whether he should be hospitalized; Rosenberg did not see him again, but later found


out that he in fact had been hospitalized for depression.


This experience brings home the limitations of relying solely on people’s self-

reports of their emotional states (Achenbach et al., 2005; Meyer, 2002). This pa-


tient said that he didn’t feel depressed, but key aspects of his behavior at home,


in particular, his diffi culty getting out of bed and his lack of energy or interest in


previously interesting activities, were clear symptoms of depression. But because the


patient didn’t realize that these behaviors were a problem, he didn’t report that he


was having those symptoms. Thus, although a patient’s self-report is important, it


has limitations. Similarly, when interviewing children, the clinician must be sensitive


to the fact that they may lack adequate insight and/or the verbal ability to be reli-


able reporters of their mental health status.


Semistructured Interviews


Because clinicians sometimes want to be sure to cover specifi c ground with their


questions, they may use a semistructured interview format, asking a list of stan-


dard questions but formulating their own follow-up questions. The follow-up


questions are based on patients’ responses to the standard questions. One set


of questions that assesses a patient’s mental state at the time of the interview is


themental status exam. In a mental status exam, the clinician asks the patient to


Some problematic behaviors may be obvious to a
mental health professional while interacting with a
patient. Other behaviors, which occur infrequently
or only in specifi c types of environments, such as
avoiding stepping on cracks (which some people
with obsessive compulsive disorder do) are less
likely to be observed. If the patient doesn’t view
these behaviors as a problem, he or she may not
report them.

Creatas/Punchstock
Free download pdf