Dissociative and Somatoform Disorders 349
media, the therapist, and general social expectations (Lilienfeld et al., 1999)? Propo-
nents of the sociocognitive model recognize that childhood trauma can—at least in
some cases—indirectly be associated with DID: it is possible that childhood trauma
can lead people to become more suggestible or more able to fantasize, which can
magnify the effects on their behavior of social interactions with a therapist (Lilienfeld
et al., 1999). In other words, dissociation and DID symptoms may be indirect results
of childhood trauma rather than direct posttraumatic results.
Proponents of the sociocognitive model point out that cultural influences,
such as the airing of the movie Sybil, may have led therapists to ask leading ques-
tions regarding DID—and may have led highly suggestible patients to follow these
leads unconsciously; such infl uences would account for the great variability in the
number of cases over time. Proponents of the posttraumatic model counter that
the increased prevalence of DID after 1976 simply refl ects improved procedures for
assessment and diagnosis. In support of their position, they point to the results of a
study that is consistent with their model and contradicts the sociocognitive model:
Women who reported having recovered memories (i.e., knowledge of prior events
about which they previously had no memory) of childhood sexual abuse were less
suggestible than women in a control group, not more suggestible, as would be pre-
dicted by the sociocognitive model (Leavitt, 1997). However, this study’s support
for the posttraumatic model isn’t as strong as it might seem at fi rst blush. Partici-
pants in the study were not DID patients, and so there may be some important dif-
ference between those who recover memories of childhood abuse and don’t develop
DID versus those who do go on to develop DID.
In sum, we do know that severe trauma can lead to dissociative disorders and
can have other adverse effects (Putnam, 1989; Putnam et al., 1995). However, we
do not know whether all of those who are diagnosed with DID have actually expe-
rienced traumatic events, nor even how severe an event must be in order to be con-
sidered “traumatic.” Similarly, experiencing a traumatic event does not specifi cally
cause DID (Kihlstrom, 2005); some people respond by developing depression or
an anxiety disorder. Further, as noted in Chapter 7, many people who experience a
traumatic event do not develop any psychological disorder.
Treating Substance Use Disorders
In general, dissociative disorders improve spontaneously, without treatment. This
is especially true of dissociative amnesia and dissociative fugue. However, clini-
cians who encounter people with these disorders have used some of the treatments
discussed below. Because dissociative disorders are so rare, few systematic studies
of treatments have been conducted—and none have attempted to determine which
treatments are most effective for a particular dissociative disorder. Thus, we con-
sider treatments for dissociative disorders in general.
Targeting Neurological Factors: Medication
In general, medication is not used to treat the symptoms of dissociative disorders
because research suggests that it is not helpful for dissociative symptoms (Sierra
et al., 2003; Simeon, Stein, & Hollander, 1998). However, people with DID may
receive medication for a comorbid disorders or for anxiety or mood symptoms that
arise in response to the dissociative symptoms.
Targeting Psychological and Social Factors:
Coping and Integration
Treatments that target the psychological factors underlying dissociative disorders
focus on three elements: (1) reinterpreting the symptoms so that they don’t create
stress or lead the patient to avoid certain situations; (2) learning additional coping
strategies to manage stress (Hunter et al., 2005); and (3) for DID patients, address-
ing the presence of alters and dissociated aspects of their memories or identities. The
fi rst two foci are similar to those for treating PTSD (Kluft, 1999; see Chapter 7).
Simply being sensitive to context or responding
differently when in different emotional states
does not mean that you have alters. For example,
one study found that people who are bilingual
responded differently to a personality test, de-
pending on which language was used for the test
(Ramírez-Esparza et al., 2006). Can you think of
reasons for this result that do not involve alters?
Ramin Talaie/Corbis