The Psychology of Gender 4th Edition

(Tuis.) #1
92 Chapter 3

for behavior that is not normative, but unique.
For example, we are more likely to infer that
a person is emotional if he or she cries during
a comedy than during a sad movie. Because
many people cry during sad movies, this be-
havior is considered normative, so crying dur-
ing a sad movie does not say anything about
an individual’s personality. Crying during a
comedy, however, is not normative and leads
to stronger trait attributions for behavior.
Thus, we are also more likely to infer aggres-
sion in a woman who uses power in her speech
than in a man who uses power in his speech
because the woman’s behavior is more unique.
Another reason that it is difficult to al-
ter stereotypes is thebacklash effect. When
people display counterstereotypical behavior,
they may be penalized. In a laboratory study,
college students competed against a confed-
erate who either outperformed them in a ste-
reotypical domain (e.g., women categorizing
pictures of toddlers) or a counterstereotypical
domain (e.g., women categorizing pictures of
football players; Rudman & Fairchild, 2004).
When losing to someone who succeeded in a
counterstereotypical domain, both female and
male participants sabotaged the confederate’s
future performance by providing unhelpful
assistance. When losing to someone who suc-
ceeded in a stereotypical domain, there was no
sabotage. It appears that people are well aware
of the backlash effect, as a subsequent experi-
ment showed that participants who succeeded
in a counterstereotypical domain tried to con-
ceal their performance. Thus, the backlash
effect serves to maintain stereotypes by penal-
izing people for counterstereotypical behavior,
dissuading people from publicizing counter-
stereotypical behavior, and by undermining
performance in counterstereotypical domains.
There are circumstances in which stereo-
types can be changed. First, it is easier to dis-
confirm stereotypical traits when the behavior

(e.g., someone told him to play with the baby)
because attentiveness to children is not consis-
tent with the male gender-role stereotype. Test
this idea yourself in Do Gender 3.4 by coming
up with stereotype-consistent and stereotype-
inconsistent behaviors and asking people to
make attributions for those behaviors.
Sometimes, when we cannot ignore
stereotype-inconsistent information, we in-
stead view the behavior as more extreme. For
example, assertiveness may be viewed as more
extreme when displayed by a woman than
byaman.Correspondent inference theory
(Jones & Davis, 1965) can explain why this
happens. According to this theory, we are
more likely tomake dispositionalattributions

DO GENDER 3.4

Attributions for
Stereotype-Consistent and
Stereotype-Inconsistent Behavior

Identify a set of five behaviors that are
stereotype consistent for men and five
behaviors that are stereotype inconsis-
tent for men. An example of a stereotype-
consistent behavior is “Joe watches football
on television.” An example of a stereotype-
inconsistent behavior is “Joe is washing the
dishes.” Now, do the same for women. An
example of a stereotype-consistent behav-
ior is “Maria is sewing a shirt.” An example
of a stereotype-inconsistent behavior is
“Maria is changing the oil in her car.”
Ask 10 men and 10 women to ex-
plain each of the behaviors. Categorize
each explanation as dispositional (due to
something about the person; a trait) or
situational (due to something about the
environment, such as luck, chance, or
the force of an external agent). It is best to
be blind to the sex of the person who gave
you the response.

M03_HELG0185_04_SE_C03.indd 92 6/21/11 12:22 PM

Free download pdf