The Psychology of Gender 4th Edition

(Tuis.) #1
Sex-Related Comparisons: Theory 155

differently. Today, there is still evidence that
parents encourage sex-typed toys, although
parents may deny it. In a study of 3- to 5-year-
old children, parents said that they encour-
aged both sex-typed and cross-sex typed toys
equally among their girls and boys (Freeman,
2007). However, the children had different
perceptions of their parents’ reactions. When
the children were asked how their parents
would react to them playing with specific toys,
a majority indicated that parents would ap-
prove of sex-typed toys (90%) and only a small
minority indicated that parents would approve
of cross-sex typed toys (26%).
An important moderator of the meta-
analysis (Lytton & Romney, 1991) was meth-
odology. Studies that included more objective
methods, such as experiments and observa-
tional studies, showed larger differences in the
way parents treated boys and girls than stud-
ies that used more subjective methods, such
as questionnaires and interviews. In other
words, parents did not report treating daugh-
ters and sons differently, but their behavior
suggested otherwise. In general, the higher-
quality studies showed larger differences in
the way parents treated daughters and sons.
More recent studies suggest other ways
in which parents may treat girls and boys dif-
ferently. For example, boys are more likely
to be physically punished than girls (Zahn-
Waxler & Polanichka, 2004). Other behaviors
may be more subtle. One observational study
showed that mothers spent more time watch-
ing boys and more time actively involved with
girls (Clearfield & Nelson, 2006). Clearfield and
Nelson concluded that parents could be send-
ing the message that it is okay for boys to be
independent whereas girls require assistance.
Lytton and Romney’s (1991) meta-
analysis also showed that parents’ differential
treatment of children seemed to decline with
the child’s age. This is not surprising because

DO GENDER 5.2

Can Perceptions Alter
Sex Differences?


  1. Ask 20 people to complete two tasks,
    one being a test of spatial skills and
    one being a test of verbal skills.
    Come up with your own two tasks.

  2. Before asking people to complete
    the tasks, randomly assign them to
    one of the following two conditions:
    Condition 1: This is the control group.
    Give no particular instructions.
    Condition 2: This is the experimental
    group. Manipulate respondents into
    perceiving that the spatial task is one
    in which females excel and the ver-
    bal task is one in which males excel.
    Think about what information you can
    provide to alter people’s perceptions.
    You might provide false statistics that
    show one sex performs better than the
    other sex. You might describe the type
    of person who excels on the task in
    masculine versus feminine terms.

  3. After people have completed the
    task, have them rate how they view
    each task on a number of scales, two
    of which are:
    Not at all
    masculine12345Very masculine
    Not at all
    feminine 12345 Very feminine
    You may include other rating scales
    so that respondents will not detect
    the items of interest. You also could
    use other terms besidesmasculine
    andfeminine, such as those that
    appear on the masculinity and
    femininity scales.

  4. Compare male and female perfor-
    mance on the two tasks in the two
    different conditions.


M05_HELG0185_04_SE_C05.indd 155 6/21/11 8:03 AM

Free download pdf