The Psychology of Gender 4th Edition

(Tuis.) #1
Communication 221

This is the first chapter in the sec-


tion on gender and relationships. Before


discussing specific aspects of men’s and


women’s friendships, romantic relation-


ships, and work relationships in the next


chapters, here I review the literature on


how women and men communicate. This


chapter focuses on both verbal and non-


verbal communication. I begin by describ-


ing the research on men’s and women’s


interaction styles in childhood and adult-


hood and the variables that influence


those styles. I then turn to the literatures


on verbal behavior—the language women


and men use—and nonverbal behavior—


touching, gazing, and smiling. Com-


munication styles have implications for


leadership and influence—who becomes


a leader, styles of leadership, and how fe-


male and male leaders are perceived. The


last aspect of communication I examine is


emotion—both experiences and expression.


I conclude the chapter by reviewing the two


most prominent explanations for the sex


differences in communication suggested—


status theory and social role theory.


Interaction Styles in Childhood


Two children are sitting quietly at a table in
the family room coloring and talking about
being best friends. A group of children are
playing soccer in the backyard, shouting at
one another to get to the ball. Who are the
children at the table? In the backyard? Boys?
Girls? Both? Can you tell?
There are certainly some differences in
the ways girls and boys play. For example,
girls are more likely to play in dyads, and

boys are more likely to play in groups. Both
girls and boys are also likely to be playing
with the same sex. From very early on, chil-
dren tend to prefer and seek out interactions
with same-sex peers. Thus same-sex play, in
and of itself, becomes a socializing agent that
ultimately leads males and females to have
different interaction styles (Maccoby, 1998).
What is the evidence for same-sex play
preferences? Do you recall playing with chil-
dren of the same sex or children of the other
sex? At what age? At ages 1 and 2, there are no
preferences for same- or other-sex peers, but
by age 3, there is a clear same-sex preference in
girls (Maccoby, 1998). A year later, boys’ same-
sex preference emerges. The preference to in-
teract with same-sex peers peaks between the
ages of 8 and 11 (Maccoby, 1998). The same-
sex play preference also appears across very dif-
ferent cultures (Munroe & Romney, 2006).
Even though girls initiate the same-sex
play preference, by age 5, the preference is
stronger in boys than girls. Boys’ groups are
more exclusionary of the other sex than are
girls’ groups. Boys view other boys who play
with girls as feminine, and boys do not tolerate
feminine behavior in another boy. It is impor-
tant for boys’ sense of masculinity to demon-
strate that they are not feminine and to reject
all associations with femininity. Girls, how-
ever, do not feel the same need to reject mas-
culinity. Girls are more accepting of masculine
behavior in another girl (Maccoby, 1998).
Children also believe others like them more if
they play with the same sex than with the other
sex. In one study, children who said that others
approved of other-sex play were more likely to
engage in other-sex play (Martin et al., 1999).
Why do children prefer to play with oth-
ers of the same sex? There are at least three
reasons (Maccoby, 1998; Mehta & Strough,
2009). First, girls and boys have different
styles of play and communication that are not

M07_HELG0185_04_SE_C07.indd 221 6/21/11 8:11 AM

Free download pdf