The Psychology of Gender 4th Edition

(Tuis.) #1
314 Chapter 9

Outside of these vignette studies, the
double standard was evaluated among sev-
enth through twelfth graders by linking a
student’s popularity to number of sexual
partners (Kreager & Staff, 2009). Popularity
was measured by having each student nomi-
nate their five best female friends and five
best male friends. Number of nominations
received was the measure of popularity. As
shown in Figure 9.6, females’ popularity was
unaffected by whether they had 0 to 8 sexual
partners, but was dramatically reduced if
they had more than 8 partners. By contrast,
the more sexual partners a male had, the
more popular he was. In fact, males with no
sexual partners were viewed as less popular
than females with no sexual partners.
Taken collectively, the double standard
seems to be alive and well, but it operates in
the more extreme cases. The double standard
is also the product of a confirmation bias—
that is, people tend to notice information
that confirms the double standard but fail to
notice information that disconfirms the dou-
ble standard (Marks & Fraley, 2006). When
respondents were provided with a vignette
with equal positive and negative statements
about a person’s sexuality, they were more
likely to recall the negative statements when
the target was female and more likely to re-
call the positive statements when the target
was male. See if a double standard exists in
your school with Do Gender 9.3.
Men not only have more permissive atti-
tudes toward sex, but men also find it more ac-
ceptable to try to attract someone else’s mate,
a phenomenon referred to as “mate poach-
ing.” In a study of nearly 17,000 people across
53 countries, more men than women admit-
ted to engaging in mate poaching and to suc-
cumbing to mate poaching—meaning that
they became involved with other women
when they were in a committed relationship
(Schmitt et al., 2004). Consistent with social

clear on this issue. A study of college students
and Internet participants were asked to evalu-
ate a female and a male target who had a vary-
ing number of sexual partners. Both female and
male participants evaluated targets with more
sexual partners more negatively—regardless of
whether the target was female or male (Marks &
Fraley, 2005). Subsequent research showed that
the double standard is a stereotype and that
stereotypes are more likely to be applied when
attention is limited. College students were
asked to evaluate a female or a male target who
had 1, 7, or 19 sexual partners (Marks, 2008).
Half of the students were asked to read the
vignette and answer the questions while re-
hearsing an eight-digit number throughout the
experiment, and the other half were not. The
double standard appeared only when people
were distracted by the rehearsal task. This con-
dition is shown in Figure 9.5. Male and female
targets with 1 and 7 partners were evaluated
similarly but female targets with 17 partners
were viewed more negatively and male targets
with 17 partners were viewed more positively.

FIGURE 9.5 When attention was divided, par-
ticipants evaluated male and female targets with 1
and 7 partners similarly. Evaluations of male tar-
gets with 19 partners increased, and evaluations
of female targets with 19 partners decreased.
Source: Adapted from Marks (2008).

Evaluation

3.9

3.5
3.3
3.1
2.9
2.7
2.5

Number of Sexual Partners

1719

3.7

Female Target

Male Target

M09_HELG0185_04_SE_C09.indd 314 6/21/11 12:40 PM

Free download pdf