costs of 5.9 percent. This is what we expect if investors are sufficiently
overconfident about the precision of their information. The first null hy-
pothesis is that this difference in returns is greater than or equal to 5.9 per-
cent. This null is consistent with rationality. The second hypothesis is that
over these same horizons the average returns to securities bought are less
than those to securities sold, ignoring trading costs. This hypothesis implies
that investors must actually misinterpret useful information. The second
null hypothesis is that average returns to securities bought are greater than
or equal to those sold.
For all three follow-up periods the average subsequent market-adjusted
returns to stocks that were bought is less than that to stocks that were
sold. Figure 15.3 provides a graph of the difference between the market-
adjusted returns to stocks that were bought and the market-adjusted re-
turns to stocks that were sold. Regardless of the horizon, the stocks that
investors bought underperformed the stocks that they sold. (This is also
true when actual returns are calculated instead of market-adjusted re-
turns.) Not only do the investors pay transactions costs to switch stocks,
but the stocks they buy underperform the ones they sell. Over a four-
month horizon, the average market-adjusted return on a purchased stock
is 1.45 percentage points lower than the average market-adjusted return
on a stock sold. Over a one-year horizon, this underperformance is 3.2
percentage points. Over a two-year horizon the shortfall is only slightly
greater, 3.6 percentage points.
INDIVIDUAL INVESTORS 557