in which five teenage boys confessed to brutally beat-
ing and raping a female jogger in New York’s Central
Park. Even though the boys were subjected to lengthy
and harsh interrogations, gave confessions that were
filled with factual errors, retracted their confessions
shortly thereafter, and were excluded as donors of the
semen by DNA tests, each was convicted at trial—
solely on the basis that they had confessed. It was not
until 13 years later that they were exonerated when a
serial rapist stepped forward from prison to confess.
His confession betrayed firsthand knowledge of the
crime and was supported by a match to the original
DNA sample.
Questions of Prevalence
The jogger case is notorious but not unique. Beginning
with the Salem witch trials of 1692, numerous false
confessions surfaced when it was later discovered that
the confessed crime had not been committed (e.g., the
alleged victim turned up alive) or that it was physically
impossible (e.g., the confessor was demonstrably else-
where) or when the real perpetrator was apprehended
(e.g., by ballistics evidence). Indeed, as more and more
wrongful convictions are discovered, often as a result of
newly available DNA tests on old evidence, it is appar-
ent that 15% to 25% of those wrongfully convicted had
confessed. Moreover, many false confessions are dis-
covered before there is a trial, are not reported by the
police, are not publicized by the media, or result in plea
bargains and are never contested—suggesting that the
known cases represent the tip of an iceberg. In short,
although it is not possible to know the prevalence rate
of false confessions, it is clear that they occur with
some regularity, making it important to understand how
they come about and how they can be prevented.
Types of False Confessions
Both criminals and innocent suspects may confess,
providing true and false confessions, respectively.
Based on a taxonomy introduced by Saul Kassin and
Lawrence Wrightsman, it is now common to further
divide the latter into three types: voluntary, compliant,
and internalized.
VVoolluunnttaarryy FFaallssee CCoonnffeessssiioonnss
In the absence of pressure from the police, volun-
tary false confessions occur when people freely admit
to crimes for which they were not responsible.
Sometimes innocent people have volunteered confes-
sions in this way to protect the actual perpetrator, often
a parent or a child. At other times, however, voluntary
false confessions have resulted from a pathological
desire for attention, especially in high-profile cases
reported in the news media; a conscious or uncon-
scious need for self-punishment to alleviate feelings of
guilt over other transgressions; or an inability to distin-
guish fact from fantasy, a common feature of certain
psychological disorders. As revealed in actual known
cases, the motives underlying voluntary false confes-
sions are as diverse as the people who make them.
A number of high-profile cases illustrate the
point. In 1932, the aviator Charles Lindbergh’s baby
was kidnapped, prompting some 200 people to vol-
unteer confessions. In 1947, Elizabeth Short, a
young, aspiring actress, later called “Black Dahlia”
for her black hair and attire, was brutally murdered
in Los Angeles and her nude body cut in half,
prompting more than 60 people, mostly men, to con-
fess. In the 1980s, Henry Lee Lucas falsely con-
fessed to hundreds of unsolved murders, mostly in
Texas, making him the most prolific serial confessor
in history. More recently, John Mark Karr was
arrested in Thailand in the summer of 2006, after it
appeared that he had voluntarily confessed to the
unsolved 1996 murder of JonBenét Ramsey, a 6-
year-old beauty pageant contestant in Boulder,
Colorado. Karr was intimately familiar with the facts
of the crime. Ultimately, he was not charged, how-
ever, after his ex-wife placed him in a different state
and after DNA tests from the crime scene implicated
another, still unidentified, man.
CCoommpplliiaanntt FFaallssee CCoonnffeessssiioonnss
In contrast to cases in which innocent people con-
fess without external pressure are the numerous false
confessions that are elicited through pressure from
family, friends, and most notably, the processes of
police interrogation.
In many of these cases, the suspect surrenders to
the demand for a confession to escape from the stress
and discomfort of the situation, avoid a threat of harm
or punishment, or gain a promised or implied reward.
This type of confession is a mere act of public compli-
ance by a suspect who comes to believe that the
short-term benefits of confession relative to denial
outweigh the long-term costs. American history
False Confessions——— 307
F-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:42 PM Page 307