Encyclopedia of Psychology and Law

(lily) #1
Scientific Research on False Memories
LLoosstt iinn tthhee MMaallll

As the notion of repression became more popular,
some psychological scientists began asking them-
selves if these “recovered memory therapy” (RMT)
techniques might be dangerous. Would it be possible,
they wondered, for people to “recover” memories for
false childhood events?
The answer was yes. In a landmark study in 1995,
Elizabeth Loftus and Jacquie Pickrell showed that
they could implant a false childhood memory using a
seemingly innocuous RMT technique: asking people
to try to remember a childhood experience. They
asked people in their study to read descriptions of four
childhood events. Three descriptions were genuine—
having been provided by a family member—and one
description was false. The false event described the
reader being lost in a shopping mall and being rescued
by an elderly lady. For example, one person in the
study read this description:

You, your mom, Tien, and Tuan all went to the
Bremerton K-Mart. You must have been 5 years old
at the time. Your mom gave each of you some money
to get a blueberry Icee. You ran ahead to get into the
line first, and somehow lost your way in the store.
Tien found you crying to an elderly Chinese woman.
You three then went together to get an Icee.

People were asked to write everything they could
remember about all four events, and then they were
interviewed twice over as much as 2 weeks. By the
end of the study, approximately 25% of the people
reported at least some information about the false
shopping mall episode. Some of the memories were
rich narratives, while others were less so—although
perhaps even these may have developed if they had
had more time to incubate.
The “Lost in the Mall” study was the first demon-
stration that everyday people could come to recall
entirely false events, a finding that showcased the
malleability of autobiographical memory and ques-
tioned the legitimacy of some of the recovered mem-
ories emerging in therapy. It also gave rise to a
number of studies using the same basic paradigm.
Since then, scientists have shown that people can
recover memories of a wide range of false experi-
ences, from being attacked by an animal to being
saved by a lifeguard.

PPhhoottooggrraapphhss
Another RMT technique is photographic review, in
which people look through photo albums as a way of trig-
gering recall of their buried abuse memories. Scientists
asked two questions about this technique: (1) Are pho-
tos powerful enough to elicit memories of false events?
(2) Do photos add power to a false suggestion?
To answer the first question, Kimberley Wade and
colleagues followed the “Lost in the Mall” procedure
but swapped the written event descriptions for pho-
tographs. Again, one of the events was fake: taking a
hot-air balloon ride. The people who took part in the
study each saw a doctored photograph of themselves
and at least one family member in the basket of a hot-
air balloon. Each person was interviewed three times
over approximately 2 weeks and asked to work at
remembering the experience. Even in the absence of
any narrative suggestion, by the end of the study, half
the subjects came to remember something about the
balloon ride. In short, photographs can lead people to
remember experiences that never really happened.
Of course, as dubious an RMT technique as photo-
graphic review might be, it does not call for the use of
doctored photos—instead, clients are encouraged to
review family albums in concert with the suggestion that
they might be repressing memories for childhood abuse.
But suppose that suggestion is false. We have already
seen that false suggestions can lead people to report
false experiences. Would the combination of a false sug-
gestion and a real childhood photo be especially danger-
ous? Stephen Lindsay and his colleagues addressed this
question by asking one group of people to read descrip-
tions about some grammar school experiences. One of
the events was false and described getting in trouble for
playing a prank on a schoolteacher. A second group also
read descriptions—including the false story about the
school prank—and received class photos corresponding
to the age at which each event took place. As in previ-
ous studies, nearly half the “descriptions-only” people
remembered something about the prank, but more than
three quarters of the “descriptions-plus-photo” people
remembered something. This study shows that the com-
bination of familiar real photos and a false suggestion
can be especially dangerous.

How Do False Memories Develop?
The scientific research now clearly shows that it
is possible to change people’s autobiographies by

False Memories——— 311

F-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:42 PM Page 311

Free download pdf