Encyclopedia of Psychology and Law

(lily) #1
further refinement of existing theories will lead to
future discoveries in the psychology of homicide.

Jennifer Pryor and
Richard L. Michalski

See also Antisocial Personality Disorder; Child
Maltreatment; Criminal Behavior, Theories of; Intimate
Partner Violence; Media Violence and Behavior;
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory–2
(MMPI–2); Mood Disorders; Psychotic Disorders

Further Readings
Bancroft, C. P. (1898). Subconscious homicide and suicide;
their physiological psychology. American Journal of
Insanity, 55,263–273.
Buss, D. M., & Duntley, J. D. (2006). The evolution of
aggression. In M. Schaller, J. A. Simpson, & D. T.
Kenrick (Eds.),Evolution and social psychology
(pp. 263–285). New York: Psychology Press.
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide.New York: Aldine
de Gruyter.
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1997). Crime and conflict:
Homicide in evolutionary psychology perspective. Crime
& Justice, 22,51–100.
Nisbett, R. E. (1993). Violence and U.S. regional culture.
American Psychologist, 48,441–449.
Smith, M. D., & Zahn, M. (2004). Homicide: A sourcebook
of social research.Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

HOPKINSCOMPETENCY


ASSESSMENTTEST(HCAT)


The Hopkins Competency Assessment Test (HCAT)
was developed as a brief screening measure for
assessing a patient’s capacity to provide informed
consent and prepare advance directives regarding
medical treatments. As mental health clinicians have
increasingly recognized the importance of accurately
assessing a patient’s ability to provide informed con-
sent, the need for measures to quantify this ability has
grown. The HCAT represents one of the first such
efforts at developing a standardized approach to eval-
uating the capacity to provide informed consent by
providing a systematic measure of comprehension.
Although primarily used in research settings, this
measure has the potential to help inform clinical judg-
ments about decision-making competence.

The HCAT, developed by Jeffrey Janofsky, consists
of a short description of the informed consent process
and the durable power of attorney, followed by six
questions (e.g., What are four things a doctor must tell
a patient before beginning a procedure?). These ques-
tions evaluate the patient’s comprehension of the infor-
mation disclosed and yield a score ranging from 0 to
10, with scores of 3 or lower signifying inadequate
comprehension. In their validation study, Janofsky and
colleagues provided interrater reliability for the HCAT
by analyzing the ratings of two independent examiners
on a series of 16 cases. Not surprisingly, given the sim-
plicity of the scoring system, the authors found a corre-
lation of .95, suggesting a high degree of consistency in
HCAT scoring. Other forms of reliability, however,
have not been analyzed and are potentially less salient.
For example, because the clinical condition of many
patients changes over time, test-retest reliability is not
necessarily a meaningful index of scale reliability.
The content validity of the HCAT has been evalu-
ated in several research studies. For example, Jeffrey S.
Janofsky and colleagues compared the results of the
HCAT with the opinion of an experienced psychiatrist
who was not shown the HCAT results. All individuals
whom the psychiatrist considered incompetent had
received a score of 3 or less on the HCAT, whereas
none of the individuals who “failed” the HCAT were
considered competent by the psychologist (i.e., a 100%
accuracy rate for determination of competence).
Barton, on the other hand, found very little concor-
dance between HCAT scores and clinician opinions
regarding competence; however, the latter were based
on hospital records indicating that a patient had been
considered incompetent (which rarely occurred).
Subsequent studies have analyzed the association
between HCAT scores and ratings of patient functional
impairment, as well as performance on other measures
of cognitive functioning. For example, Sorger et al.
(2007) found markedly poorer decision-making abil-
ity, based on the HCAT, among elderly patients diag-
nosed with terminal cancer compared with a
physically healthy elderly sample, even after control-
ling for other group differences (e.g., age, gender, etc).
Nearly half (44%) of the terminally ill patients studied
“failed” the HCAT compared with only 6% of
an ambulatory nursing home comparison sample.
Moreover, HCAT scores were significantly correlated
with other measures of cognitive functioning including
the Mini-Mental State Exam.
Despite strong preliminary data in support of the reli-
ability and validity of the HCAT, this measure is rarely

Hopkins Competency Assessment Test (HCAT)——— 359

H-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:43 PM Page 359

Free download pdf