Encyclopedia of Psychology and Law

(lily) #1
session, where they will be sober when they complete the
dependent measures. Regardless of the research ques-
tion, for safety purposes, all participants in this type of
research must be relatively sober before they are permit-
ted to leave the research lab. To ensure that participants’
BAC is low enough for them to be excused (usually
0.03% or lower, as set by individual institutional review
boards), a breathalyzer is used. It should be noted that
although a blood-test analysis could also be conducted in
lieu of a breathalyzer, this practice is not normally used
by psychology and law researchers. Also, laboratory
research is limited with regard to the amount of alcohol
that can be safely administered to participants. Although
there may be exceptions depending on the location
(country) of data collection, the research question, and
individual IRBs, generating BACs in the lab greater than
0.08% is rarely permitted.

FFiieelldd SSttuuddiieess
Field studies, on the other hand, do not normally
screen participants for characteristics that would make
them ineligible for lab studies because participants in
field studies are obtained in bars or drinking establish-
ments and have consumed alcohol, presumably on
their own volition, prior to taking part in the research
study. Also, because participants have consumed alco-
hol on their own, obtaining participants with BACs
higher than 0.08% is common. Overall, there are few
differences between field and lab research with regard
to the presentation of stimuli or measuring of depen-
dent variables. One important difference, however,
should be noted. Due to the fact that participants in
field studies are intoxicated at the time when consent
to participate is given, they must be provided with an
opportunity to withdraw their participation at a later
time (i.e., when they are sober).

Intoxicated Eyewitnesses:
Experimental Findings
Researchers have been examining the effects of alco-
hol on eyewitness memory since the early 1990s.
Early experiments examined the effects of alcohol on
memory by comparing groups that were either sober
or intoxicated at the time of encoding and then testing
all participants on a different day when all participants
were sober. The results from these studies suggested
that intoxicated participants were less accurate when
asked to recall the features of a target person and less
accurate about describing the events that took place

during the critical encoding period than were sober
witnesses. However, participants who were intoxicated
during encoding were just as accurate at identifying a
target person in an identification task as witnesses who
had not consumed alcohol. Although these studies were
not specifically testing alcohol myopia theory, the
results are consistent with alcohol myopia theory
predictions.
Later research examined the effects of alcohol intox-
ication at the time of encoding and at the identification
task. Although it is possible to conduct this research by
having participants return to the lab a second time to
become intoxicated again (i.e., context reinstatement),
this body of research administered the dependent vari-
ables (e.g., a showup) relatively soon after the viewing
of the target person and while the participants were still
intoxicated. This research was unique from earlier
studies in that it allowed researchers to study alcohol
myopia theory by manipulating (a) the behavior of the
investigator and (b) the identification procedure. This
research was relevant to real police practice because the
police often encounter intoxicated individuals in the
course of their investigations and there had been no
research on the potential vulnerabilities of intoxicated
witnesses to police practices. The findings of these
studies suggest that intoxicated participants are more
susceptible to minor changes in police procedure, such
as the instructions that are given to a witness prior to
viewing a showup (e.g., “Please be careful when mak-
ing your decision.”) and biased identification proce-
dures (e.g., when the suspect is shown wearing similar
clothes to those worn by the perpetrator). Ultimately,
however, intoxicated witnesses could, under the cir-
cumstances of these research studies, be more accurate
than sober witnesses. In addition, correct identification
decision rates were in the neighborhood of 90%—a
notably high rate even for sober witnesses in eyewit-
ness identification research.

Jennifer E. Dysart

See also Eyewitness Descriptions, Accuracy of; Eyewitness
Memory; Identification Tests, Best Practices in

Further Readings
Dysart, J. E., Lindsay, R. C. L., MacDonald, T. K., & Wicke,
C. (2002). The intoxicated witness: Effects of alcohol on
identification accuracy. Journal of Applied Psychology,
87,170–175.
Read, J. D., Yuille, J. C., & Tollestrup, P. (1992).
Recollections of a robbery: Effects of arousal and alcohol

Alcohol Intoxication, Impact on Eyewitness Memory——— 13

A-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:41 PM Page 13

Free download pdf