The Economist - USA (2022-01-22)

(Antfer) #1

72 Science & technology The Economist January 22nd 2022


onesduringyouth.Repairsneed notbe
perfect—just successful enough to keep
theshowontheroad.Anti­cancermecha­
nismsneedtobetip­topforthefirstde­
cadesoflife,butcangetslackerwithtime.
Ascantheimmunesystem.Thoughthey
will,nodoubt,buildoutwardfromtheir
starting point, Altos’s researchers will
surelyhavetoincorporatemoreaspectsof
molecularbiologythanthosetheyarebe­
ginningwith,inordertocoverthesebases.
The counterargument, put by Dr
Klausnerandhiscolleagues,isthatreset­
tingtheclockisa naturalprocess.Ithap­
penseverygeneration.The reproductive
cellswhichcreatethesenewgenerations
geta freshstarteachtime.Theyreallydo
returntofactorysettings.Andiftheclock
canberesetforthosecells,whynotothers?
WhetherDrMilner,MrNelsenandtheoth­
erswhohavebackedthefirmseea return
ontheirinvestmentwilldepend,aboveall,
ontheanswertothatquestion.Butitwill
befascinatingtoseeit asked.n

Childpsychology

Drools of


attraction


T


hecomplexitiesofhumanrelations
are difficult enough for adults to navi­
gate—and  they  have  at  least  some  idea  of
the rules. Children have yet to learn those
rules. Infants are, nonetheless, able quick­
ly  to  identify  close  relationships  between
other people, and thus to build up a map of
the social world around them. How they do
this has perplexed sociologists, anthropol­
ogists  and  developmental  psychologists
for  decades.  In  a  paper  just  published  in

Science,  Ashley  Thomas  of  the  Massachu­
setts  Institute  of  Technology  proposes  a
partial answer: slobber.
To avoid the sexual connotations of the
word “intimacy”, Dr Thomas and her team
refer to the “thickness” of interactions be­
tween  infants  and  adults—borrowing  the
term from Avishai Margalit, a philosopher.
Thick relationships involve strong attach­
ments,  obligations  and  mutual  respon­
siveness. One set of cues for thick relation­
ships relates to things that involve sharing
saliva:  kissing,  for  example,  or  the  com­
mon use of an eating or drinking utensil. 
To test whether children interpret sali­
va­sharing  as  indicating  a  thick  relation­
ship, the researchers recruited two groups
of several dozen youngsters. One was a set
of  babies  aged  between  eight  and  ten
months. The other was a group of toddlers
aged  between  16  and  18  months.  To  avoid
the hazards of covid­19, all tests were con­
ducted over a video link. 
Each child was shown a clip of an adult
interacting  with  a  puppet,  followed  by  a
clip  of  that  puppet  in  distress  while  the
same adult, and also a stranger, looked on.
When  the  interaction  in  the  first  clip  ap­
peared  to  involve  the  sharing  of  saliva—
with puppet and adult portrayed as taking
consecutive  bites  from  an  orange—both
sets of children looked mainly at that same
adult in the second clip, and not the strang­
er, a reaction interpreted as a belief that the
adult  in  question  would  offer  comfort  to
the  puppet.  When  the  interaction  in  the
first  clip  was  friendly  but  less  thick,  such
as  passing  a  ball  back  and  forth,  the  chil­
dren had equal expectations of both adults
when  shown  the  second  clip.  Saliva  shar­
ing seems, then, indicative of closeness.
That conclusion was reinforced by sub­
sequently replacing the puppet with a dif­
ferent  one  and  repeating  the  second  test.
In this case the children showed no consis­
tent expectation about which adult would
intervene to relieve the puppet’s distress. It
thus seems to have been the act of sharing
an orange with a specific puppet that trig­
gered  an  expectation  of  future  behaviour,
rather than any inherent characteristics of
the adults involved.
Conducting  her  experiment  by  video
enabled  Dr  Thomas  to  cast  her  search  for
trial  participants  beyond  Massachusetts.
She  nevertheless  decided,  in  this  first  in­
stance,  to  confine  things  to  the  United
States.  Future  runs,  she  hopes,  will  reach
beyond that country’s borders. 
The  ethnographic  literature  suggests
saliva­sharing  is  a  widespread  phenome­
non. It also makes sense as a signal of inti­
macy, for its disease­spreading potential is
obvious and engaging in it therefore indi­
cates a high degree of trust between partic­
ipants. But seeing how practice variesfrom
place to place (if, indeed, it does),might il­
luminate some intriguing details.n

Babies learn about people by looking
at who shares saliva

Thick as thieves

Tropicaldiseases

Resistance


is useless


A


n arms race between  pharmacolo­
gists  and  malaria  parasites  has  been
going on since the mid­19th century, when
widespread use of quinine began. Few bet­
ter  illustrations  of  natural  selection  exist
than the repeated emergence of resistance
to such drugs. Even artemisinin, the most
recent addition to the arsenal, has already
provoked an evolutionary pushback. 
At  the  moment,  working  out  which
drugs, if any, a particular case of malaria is
resistant  to  means  sending  a  sample  to  a
laboratory  for  a  pcrtest.  But  malaria  is
most  often  a  problem  in  poor  countries,
where such laboratories are scarce, and so
is  money  to  pay  for  tests  and  to  maintain
the  machines  needed  to  conduct  them.  A
better  way  for  doctors  and  paramedics  in
the  field  to  be  able  to  tell,  for  a  particular
patient, which drugs the infection is resis­
tant  to  would  thus  be  welcome.  And  that
may  soon  be  possible,  thanks  to  work  by
Ron Dzikowski and Eylon Yavin of the He­
brew  University  in  Jerusalem.  As  they
write  in  acsSensors,  they  have  come  up
with  a  trick  which  they  think  could  be
turned into a cheap and deployable detec­
tor for drug resistance.
The term “malaria” covers several simi­
lar  illnesses  caused  by  single­celled  para­
sites of the genus Plasmodium. The deadli­
est,  Plasmodium falciparum,  kills  around
600,000  people  a  year,  80%  of  them  chil­
dren under five. Dr Dzikowski and Dr Yavin
therefore focused their attention on this. 
They knew from research by others that
many of the drug­resistant traits in P. falci-

A field test for drug-resistant malarial
parasites will help save lives

Little bleeder
Free download pdf