References 79
not reflect the way personality is organized at a genetic level.
An equally feasible structure would involve considerable dif-
ferences in complexity across domains. Some domains may
consist of a relatively large number of traits, whereas others
may consist of only one or two genetically homogeneous
traits. These assumptions are consistent with the lexical view
of Saucier and Goldberg (1996), who argued that the five do-
mains are merely a convenient way of organizing lower-order
traits and that there is no inherent reason to assume that do-
mains are equal in breadth or in pervasiveness.
Although behavioral genetic analyses show that environ-
mental factors exert a considerable influence on personality,
they do not appear to influence the structural relationships
among traits to any appreciable extent. Instead, environ-
mental factors appear to exert a more contemporaneous effect
on trait expression. The nature of these factors and the way
that they function remain important topics of research.
REFERENCES
Allport, G. W. (1961). Pattern and growth in personality.New York:
Holt, Reinhart and Winston.
Almagor, M., Tellegen, A., & Waller, N. G. (1995). The big seven
model: A cross-cultural replication and further exploration of the
basic dimensions of natural language trait descriptors. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 69,300–307.
American Psychiatric Association (1994). The diagnostic and statis-
tical manual for mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC:
Author.
Ashton, M. C., Lee, K., Vernon, P. A., & Jang, K. L. (2000). Fluid
intelligence, crystallized intelligence, and the Openness/Intellect
factor. Journal of Research in Personality, 34,198–207.
Avia, M. D., Sanz, J., Sanchez-Bernardos, M. L., Martinez-Arias,
M. R. Silva, & Graña, (1995). The five-factor model—II. Rela-
tions of the NEO-PI with other personality variables. Personal-
ity and Individual Differences, 19,81–97.
Baker, L. A., & Daniels, D. (1990). Nonshared environmental influ-
ences and personality differences in adult twins. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 58,103–110.
Benjamin, J., Greenberg, B., & Murphy, D. L. (1996). Mapping per-
sonality traits related to genes: Population and family association
between the D4 dopamine receptor and measures of novelty
seeking.Nature Genetics, 12,81–84.
Benjamin, J., Osher, Y., Lichtenberg, P., Bachner-Melman, R.,
Gritsenko, I., Kotler, M., Belmaker, R. H., Valsky, V., Drendel,
M., & Ebstein, R. P. (2000). An interaction between the catechol
O-methyltransferase and serotonin transporter promoter region
polymorphisms contributes to Tridimensional Personality Ques-
tionnaire persistence scores in normal subjects. Neuropsycho-
biology, 41,48–53.
Bergeman, C. S., Chipeur, H. M., Plomin, R., Pederson, N. L.,
McClearn, G. E., Nesselroade, J. R., Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R.
(1993). Genetic and environmental effects on openness to experi-
ence, agreeableness, and conscientiousness: An adoption/twin
study.Journal of Personality, 61,159–179.
Boomsma, D. I. (1996). Using multivariate genetic modeling to
detect pleiotropic quantitative trait loci. Behavior Genetics, 26,
161–166.
Bouchard, T. J. (1999). Genes, environment, and personality. In S. J.
Ceci & W. M. Williams (Eds.), The nature-nurture debate: The
essential readings. Essential readings in developmental psychol-
ogy(pp. 97–103). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Carey, G. (1987). Is extraversion a unitary trait? Behavior Genetics,
17,619.
Carey, G., & DiLalla, D. L. (1994). Personality and psychopathol-
ogy: Genetic perspectives. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
103,32–43.
Caspi, A., & Bem, D. J. (1990). Personality continuity and change
across the life course. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of per-
sonality: Theory and research.New York: Guilford.
Casto, S. D., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1995). Multivariate
genetic analysis of Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—
Revised (WISC–R) factors. Behavior Genetics, 25,25–32.
Cloninger, C. R. (1987). A systematic method for clinical descrip-
tion and classification of personality variants: A proposal.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 44,573–588.
Cloninger, C. R., Adolfsson, R., & Svrakic, N. M. (1996). Mapping
genes for human personality. Nature Genetics, 12,3–4.
Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Przybeck, T. R. (1993). A psy-
chobiological model of temperament and character. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 50, 975–990.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). Manual for the NEO Person-
ality Inventory. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment
Resources.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality
Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and the NEO Five-Factor Inventory
(NEO-FFI) professional manual.Odessa, FL: Psychological
Assessment Resources.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1995). Domains and facets: Hierar-
chical personality assessment using the Revised NEO Personal-
ity Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 64, 21–50.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1997). Longitudinal stability of adult
personality. In R. Hogan & J. A. Johnson (Eds.), Handbook
of personality psychology (pp. 269–290). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1998). Six approaches to the explica-
tion of facet-level traits: Examples from conscientiousness.
European Journal of Psychology, 12,117–134.
Crawford, C. B., & DeFries, J. C. (1978). Factor analysis of genetic
and environmental correlation matrices. Multivariate Behavioral
Research, 13,297–318.