278 Social Cognition
Bargh, J. A. (1999). The cognitive monster: The case against the
controllability of automatic stereotype effects. In S. Chaiken &
Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories in social psychology
(pp. 361–382). New York: Guilford.
Bargh, J. A., & Barndollar, K. (1996). Automaticity in action: The
unconscious as the repository of chronic goals and motives. In
P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action:
Linking cognition and motivation to behavior(pp. 457–481).
New York: Guilford.
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automatic-
ity of being. American Psychologist, 54,462–479.
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (2000). The mind in the middle: A
practical guide to priming and automaticity research. In H. T.
Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in
social and personality psychology(pp. 253–285). Cambridge,
England: Cambridge University Press.
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of
social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype
activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 71,230–244.
Bargh, J. A., & Ferguson, M. J. (2000). Beyond behaviorism: On the
automaticity of higher mental processes. Psychological Bulletin,
126,925–945.
Bargh, J. A., & Pietromonaco, P. (1982). Automatic information
processing and social perception: The impact of trait information
presented outside of conscious awareness on impression for-
mation.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43,
437–449.
Baron, R. S., Kerr, N. L., & Miller, N. (1992). Group process, group
decision, group action.Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks Cole.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and
theory of mind.Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bartlett, F. A. (1932). A study in experimental and social psychol-
ogy.New York: Cambridge University Press.
Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of
sex typing. Psychological Review, 88,354–364.
Bem, S. L. (1993).The lenses of gender: Transforming the debate
on sexual inequality.New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Blanchard, F. A., Lilly, T., & Vaughn, L. A. (1991). Reducing the ex-
pression of racial prejudice. Psychological Science, 2,101–105.
Bodenhausen, G. V. (1990). Stereotypes a judgmental heuristics:
Evidence of circadian variations in discrimination. Psychologi-
cal Science, 1,319–322.
Bodenhausen, G. V. (1992). Information-processing functions of
generic knowledge structures and their role in context effects in
social judgment. In N. Schwarz & S. Sudman (Eds.), Context
effects in social and psychological research(pp. 267–277).
New York: Springer-Verlag.
Bodenhausen, G. V., Kramer, G. P., & Süsser, K. (1994). Happiness
and stereotypic thinking in social judgment. Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology, 66,621–632.
Bodenhausen, G. V., Mussweiler, T., Gabriel, S., & Moreno, K. N.
(2001). Affective influences on stereotyping and intergroup rela-
tions. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Handbook of affect and social cogni-
tion(pp. 319–343). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1971). The abstraction of linguistic
ideas.Cognitive Psychology, 2,331–350.
Brewer, M. B. (1997). On the social origins of human nature. In
C. McGarty & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The message of social psy-
chology(pp. 54–62). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Bruner, J. S. (1957). On perceptual readiness. Psychological Re-
view, 64,123–152.
Byrne, R. W. (2000). Evolution of primate cognition. Cognitive
Science, 24,543–570.
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J. A., & Jarvis, W. B. G.
(1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The
life and times of individuals varying in the need for cognition.
Psychological Bulletin, 119,197–253.
Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (Eds.). (1999). Dual process theories in
social psychology.New York: Guilford.
Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1997). Nonconscious behavioral confir-
mation processes: The self-fulfilling consequences of automatic
stereotype activation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol-
ogy, 33,541–560.
Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., & Conway, M. (1994). Affective causes
and consequences of social information processing. In. R. S.
Wyer, Jr., & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition
(2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 323–417). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cummins, D. D. (1998). Social norms and other minds: The evolu-
tionary roots of higher cognition. In D. D. Cummins & C. Allen
(Eds.),The evolution of mind(pp. 30–50). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic
and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 56,5–18.
Ditto, P. H., & Lopez, D. F. (1992). Motivated skepticism: Use of
differential decision criteria for preferred and nonpreferred
conclusions.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34,
568–584.
Dodge, K. A. (1993). Social-cognitive mechanisms in the develop-
ment of conduct disorder and depression. Annual Review of
Psychology, 44,559–584.
Donders, F. C. (1868). Die Schnelligkeit psychischer Processe
[The speed of mental processes]. Archiv für Anatomie und
Wissenschaftliche Medizin,657–681.
Dovidio, J. F., Evans, N., & Tyler, R. B. (1986). Racial stereotypes:
The contents of their cognitive representations. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 22,22–37.
de Dreu, C. K. W., Koole, S. L., & Oldersma, F. L. (1999). On the
seizing and freezing of negotiator inferences: Need for cognitive
closure moderates the use of heuristics in negotiation. Personal-
ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25,348–362.