Coren, S., & Suedfeld, P. (1995). Personality correlates of conceptual complexity.Journal of So-
cial Behavior and Personality, 10 , 229–242.
Damasio, A. R. (1995).Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Avon
Books.
Davis, J. A., & Smith, T. W. (1995).General social surveys, 1972–1994: Cumulative file (ICPSR
6217)[Electronic database]. Ann Arbor, MI: International Consortium of Political and So-
cial Research [Producer and Distributor].
Dennet, D. C. (1987).The intentional stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Diehl, M., Coyle, N., & Labouvie-Vief, G. (1996). Age and sex differences in strategies of coping
and defense across the life span.Psychology and Aging, 11 , 127–139.
Dornic, S., Ekehammar, B., & Laaksonen, T. (1991). Tolerance for mental effort: Self-ratings re-
lated to perception, performance, and personality.Personality & Individual Differences, 12 ,
313–319.
Duffy, E. (1957). The psychological significance of the concept of “arousal” or “activation.”
Psychological Review, 64 , 265–275.
Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of be-
havior.Psychological Review, 66 , 183–201.
Eisdorfer, C. (1968). Arousal and performance: Experiments in verbal learning and a tentative
theory. In G. A. Talland (Ed.),Human aging and behavior: Recent advances in research and
theory(pp. 189–216). New York: Academic Press.
Ekman, P. (1984). Expression and the nature of emotion. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.),
Approaches to emotion(pp. 319–343). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and psychodynamic unconscious.American Psy-
chologist, 49 , 709–724.
Epstein, S., & Pacini, R. (1999). Some basic issues regarding dual-process theories from the per-
spective of cognitive-experiential self theory. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.),Dual-process
theories in social psychology(pp. 462–482). New York: Guilford.
Erikson, E. H. (1984).The life cycle completed: A review. New York: Norton.
Eysenck, M. W. (1982).Attention and arousal: Cognition and performance. New York: Springer-
Verlag.
Fischer, K. W., & Ayoub, C. (1994). Affective splitting and dissociation in normal and mal-
treated children: Developmental pathways for self in relationships. In D. Cicchetti & S. L.
Toth (Eds.),Disorders and dysfunctions of the self(pp. 147–222). Rochester, NY: University
of Rochester Press.
Fischer, K. W., Kenny, S., & Pipp, S. (1990). How cognitive processes and environmental condi-
tions organize discontinuities in the development of abstractions. In C. N. Alexander & E. J.
Langer (Eds.),Higher stages of human development: Perspectives on adult growth(pp.
162–187). New York: Oxford University Press.
Follett, K., & Hess, T. M. (2002). Aging, cognitive complexity, and the fundamental attribution
error.Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57B,
P312–P323.
Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E. L., & Target, M. (2002).Affect regulation, mentalization, and
the development of the self. New York: Other Press.
Forgas, J. P. (2001). The affect infusion model (AIM): An integrative theory of mood effects on
cognition and judgements. In L. L. Martin & G. L. Clore (Eds.),Theories of mood and cogni-
tion: A user’s guidebook(pp. 99–134). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions?Review of General Psychology, 2 ,
300–319.
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-
and-build theory of positive emotions.American Psychologist, 56 , 218–226.
Freud, S. (1963). Some psychological consequences of the anatomical differences between the
sexes. In P. Rieff (Ed.),Sexuality and the psychology of love(pp. 183–193). New York:
Macmillan. (Original work published 1925)
266 LABOUVIE-VIEF AND GONZÁLEZ