Bruce Richman
Abstract
Given that the most basic function in any spoken language must be the ability
of speakers to repeat utterances in the same way, exactly and precisely, and given
that human language probably began with rich, diverse, long sequences from
which meaningful utterances were selected (beginning with impoverished, short
utterances begs the question about what natural selection had to work with—
natural selection selects, but not from nothing!), a basic problem for language
origins is how sequences became fixed for entire communities, so that everyone
could repeat them exactly. Music making, in all human communities, is also based
on the ability to repeat sequences exactly. I suggest that the original process of
fixing sequences into recognizable, repeatable, and significant definite some-
things by entire communities was accomplished by speech and music making in
exactly the same way and by the same means. In other words, in the beginning,
speech and music making were one and the same: they were collective, real-time
repetitions of formulaic sequences.
As long as I have been studying gelada monkey “friendly” sounds (which
has been on and off since 1969), they have struck me as being astound-
ingly like human conversation in their passion and complexity. They
function as a kind of vocal grooming, allowing pairs of geladas to estab-
lish temporary, exclusive social bonds with each other. But that is also
the main function of most of present-day human conversational speech
(see Dunbar 1996). Also, the production units of friendly series
sequences are similar in overall form and vocal detail to human vocal
formulas (Richman 1996). People and gelada monkeys seem to have a
remarkably similar relationship to their vocalizing: they both desperately
need to establish continuing vocal relationships with a variety of con-
specifics, and they both seem to spend huge amounts of emotional and
vocal energy engaging in special kinds of friendly vocalizing to achieve
these ends.
Also, for people and geladas, all their constant friendly vocalizing,
which for both species is rich in vocal pattern variety, succeeds like
social grooming in setting up minute-to-minute relationships despite
(and because) of all the emotional and social conflicts they bring to each
encounter. Extensive friendly vocalizing relaxes and dispels some of the
tensions for participants in these conflict-filled encounters, and allows
both people and geladas to continue the system of exclusive (almost
jealous) relationships so crucial to their societies, despite the normal
background of conflicts engendered by their intense group lives.
In both cases, friendly vocalizing is produced in units averaging a total
length of about nine or ten syllables, produced at a rate of about five
syllables per second, organized by differentiation of strong and weak
17
How Music Fixed “Nonsense” into Significant Formulas:
On Rhythm, Repetition, and Meaning