The Dictionary of Human Geography

(nextflipdebug2) #1

Comp. by: LElumalai Stage : Revises1 ChapterID: 9781405132879_4_C Date:31/3/09
Time:21:45:53 Filepath://ppdys1108/BlackwellCup/00_Blackwell/00_3B2/Gregory-
9781405132879/appln/3B2/revises/9781405132879_4_C.3d


agenda for research includedpopulationand
demography,land use, patterns of home
ownership andmigration, community devel-
opment and character, neighbourhood history,
occupational and class mobility, social unrest
and social control (includingpolicingand
urban policies). Many of these topics remain
of vital interest to urban geographers, and
draw upon ideas from the Chicago School
about pattern, process and community,
although our contemporary approaches to
and theories of these topics are necessarily
different. dgm

Suggested reading
Dear (2002); Entrikin (1980); Jackson and Smith
(1984); Park, Burgess and McKenzie (1925).

children A burgeoning area of scholarship
in human geography that encompasses
notions of children as active producers of
space, as geographical subjects and as envir-
onmental agents, at the same time as it recog-
nizes children’s limited mobility, the
peculiarities of their exposure to various envir-
onmental degradations andhazards, and the
mediated nature of their spatial engagements.
The earliest work in the field, pioneered by
James Blaut and the psychologist David Stea,
addressed the ‘ontogeny of environmental
behavior’ by looking at children’s geographical
learning, especially their understandings of
spatial relationships, mapping skills andplace
knowledge (Blaut and Stea, 1971, p. 387).
Their Place Perception Project (1968–71)
spurred much generative work in the field,
including Roger Hart’s (1979) landmark study
of children’s place experience, Denis Wood’s
fascinating research on the relationship
between young people’s spatial behaviour
and their cognitivemaps, and research on such
issues as children’s differentiated access to the
outdoor environment or ‘home range,’ their
understanding of environmental processes
and human–environment interactions, and
their ability to negotiate aerial photographs.
At about the same time as Blaut, Stea and
their colleagues at Clark University were
researching children’s acquisition of environ-
mental knowledge, William Bunge (see Bunge
et al., 1971) was launching the Detroit
Geographical Expedition, which examined
the effects of noxious and deteriorated envi-
ronments on children’s well-being, and devel-
oped projects of environmental activism
around the uneven geographies of people’s
everyday lives and children’s exposure to
problems rooted in these geographies. These

two streams of work – not, coincidentally,
by radical geographers – set the stage for
much subsequent scholarship on children’s
geographies and the geographies of children,
even as some of their key practitioners were
marginalized from the field.
As geographers have continued to address
the development of spatial cognition, mapping
skills and environmental learning, there has
been an ongoing debate about whether map-
ping represents a cultural universal that chil-
dren share from earliest childhood, as Blaut
and Stea and their colleagues have argued,
or is dependent on cognitive development,
as Roger Downs, Lynn Liben and their
colleagues have argued. While much of this
debate has concerned children’s relative pre-
paredness for acquiring spatial concepts and
mapping skills (and thus was an argument
about the role of geographicaleducationat
different ages), it was animated by the princi-
pals’ understandings of the nature of Piagetian
developmental psychology and what Blaut
considered itsidealistunderpinnings. Both
sides recognized children’s embrace of geo-
graphical concepts, and advocated their being
taught mapping and spatial skills in all phases
of their education, agreeing that it would not
only be a cornerstone of enhanced geographic
literacy but contribute to cognitive develop-
ment and learning in other arenas (Blaut,
1997; Liben and Downs, 1997; cf. Matthews,
1992). Despite this conclusion and the dec-
ades of scholarship that support it, there
remains a surprising disconnection between
the work of scholars interested in geographical
or environmentaleducationand those who
look at children’s place experience and their
acquisition of environmental knowledge and
spatial skills.
Work on children’s geographies has been
developmental, ecological, milieu focused,
comparative and concerned mostly with
the global North. As the field evolved, its
concerns expanded to include children’s
understanding and experience of place (Hart,
1979; Matthews, 1992; Wood and Beck,
1994), their knowledge of environmental pro-
cesses and human–environment relations
(Kates and Katz, 1977; Hart, 1997; Katz,
2004), the social ecologies of their environmen-
tal interactions (Ruddick, 1996; Valentine,
1997; Aitken, 2001) and studies of particular
children’s environments, such as playgrounds,
schools, parks and neighbourhoods (e.g.
Skelton and Valentine, 1998; McKendrick,
1999). In tandem with broader disciplinary
concerns, research focused on children and

Gregory / The Dictionary of Human Geography 9781405132879_4_C Final Proof page 80 31.3.2009 9:45pm

CHILDREN
Free download pdf