Handbook of Psychology, Volume 4: Experimental Psychology

(Axel Boer) #1
Models of Eye Movement Control 567

culty. It attempted to account for the reading comprehension
processes ranging from individual eye fixations to the
integration of words into sentence context (e.g., clauses).
Although it was a comprehensive and highly flexible model
of reading, its relatively nebulous nature made it difficult for
researchers to use the model to make specific predictions
about the reading process.
In the past few years, however, a number of models have
been proposed that have been generally designed to expand
upon models of word perception and specifically designed to
explain and predict eye movement behavior during fluent
reading. Because these models are based upon the relatively
observable behavior of the eyes, they allow researchers to
make specific predictions about the reading process. How-
ever, as with many issues in reading, the nature of eye
movement models is a matter of controversy. Eye movement
models can be separated into two general categories: oculo-
motormodels (e.g., O’Regan, 1990, 1992), which posit that
eye movements are primarily controlled by low-level me-
chanical (oculomotor) factors and are only indirectly related
to ongoing language processing; and processing models
(Morrison, 1984; Henderson & Ferreira, 1990; Just &
Carpenter, 1980; Pollatsek, Reichle, & Rayner, in press; Re-
ichle et al., 1998; Reichle, Rayner, & Pollatsek, 1999), which
presume that lexical and other moment-to-moment cognitive
processing are important influences on when the eyes move.
Although space prohibits an extensive discussion of the pros
and cons of each of these models, in this section we briefly
delineate some of the more influential contributions to the
field.
According to oculomotor models, the decision of where to
move the eyes is determined both by visual properties of text
(e.g., word length, spaces between words) and by the limita-
tions in visual acuity that we discussed in a previous section.
Furthermore, the length of time spent actually viewing any
given word is postulated to be primarily a function of where
the eyes have landed within the word. That is to say, the loca-
tion of our fixations within words is not random. Instead,
there is a preferred viewing location—as we read, our eyes
tend to land somewhere between the middle and the
beginning of words (Radach & McConkie, 1998; Rayner,
1979). Vitu (1991) also found that although readers’ eyes
tended to land on or near this preferred viewing location,
when they viewed longer words (101 letters), readers initially
fixated near the beginning of the word and then made another
fixation near the end of the word (Rayner & Morris, 1992).
One of the more prominent oculomotor models is
thestrategy-tactics model (O’Regan, 1990, 1992; Reilly &
O’Regan, 1998). The model accounts for the aforementioned
landing position effects by stipulating that words are most
easily identified when they are fixated just to the left of the


middle of the word, but that readers may adopt one of two
possible reading strategies—one riskier, so to speak, than the
other. According to the risky strategy, readers can just try to
move their eyes so that they fixate on this optimal viewing
position within each word. In addition, readers may also use
a more careful strategy, so that when their eyes land on a
nonoptimal location (e.g., too far toward the end of the
word), they can refixate and move their eyes to the other end
of the word.
Without going into too much detail, the strategy-tactics
models make some specific predictions about the nature of
eye movements during reading. For example, they predict
that the probability of a reader’s refixating a word should
only be a function of low-level visual factors (such as where
the eyes landed in the word) and that it should not be influ-
enced by linguistic processing. However, Rayner et al.
(1996) found that the probability of a refixation was higher
for words of lower frequency than for words of higher
frequency even when the length of the two words was
matched. Due to this and other difficulties, many researchers
believe that oculomotor models are incomplete and that, al-
though they do give good explanations of how lower level
oculomotor factors influence reading, they largely ignore the
influence of linguistic factors such as word frequency and
word predictability.
As we discussed earlier, readers’ eye movements are
influenced by factors other than just word frequency (e.g.,
predictability, context, etc.). Given the influence of these lin-
guistic variables, some researchers have developed models
that are based upon the assumption that eye movements are
influenced by both lexical (linguistic) factors and by mo-
ment-to-moment comprehension processes. It should be
noted that these models generally do not exclude the influ-
ence of the low-level oculomotor strategies inherent in
oculomotor models, but they posit that this influence is small
relative to that of cognitive factors. Overall, then, processing
theorists posit that the decision of when to move the eyes
(fixation duration) is primarily a function of linguistic-
cognitive processing, and the decision of where to move the
eyes is a function of visual factors.
Although a number of models (e.g., Morrison, 1984) have
utilized such a framework, the most recent and extensive at-
tempt to predict eye movement behavior during reading is the
E-Z Reader model (Reichle & Rayner, 2001; Reichle et al.,
1998; Reichle et al., 1999). Currently, E-Z reader includes a
number of variables that have been found to influence both
fixation durations and fixation locations. Importantly, its
computational framework has been used to both simulate and
predict eye movement behavior. Although the E-Z Reader
model is complex, it essentially consists of four processes: a
familiarity check, the completion of lexical access (i.e., word
Free download pdf