Handbook of Psychology, Volume 4: Experimental Psychology

(Axel Boer) #1
References 55

evolutionary problems the animal must solve, we protect our-
selves from explanations that assume an unlimited number of
“motivations,” as did the old theories of instinct. In addition,
this focus on the functional aspects of motivational processes
forces us to consider both the ecological niche that the animal
occupies and the organization of the behaviors it uses to cope
with the problems of the niche.
This explicitly ecological view allows the concept of mo-
tivation to make contact with behavioral ecology and evolu-
tion. Learning and genetics are not the sole determinants of
behavior; an animal’s ecological niche must also be consid-
ered. Animals have evolved solutions to specific environ-
mental problems, and an understanding of these relationships
can inform psychological theories of motivation and learn-
ing. Collier and Johnson (1990) suggested that appreciating
that small predators are themselves potential prey gives in-
sight into the differences in feeding rate between small and
large predators. Indeed, Fanselow et al. (1988) have demon-
strated that predatory risk is an important determinant in the
initiation of feeding behavior. Traditional homeostatic per-
spectives could not contribute this insight.
In addition to highlighting the importance of ecological
variables in determining motivational influences on behavior,
the analyses presented in this chapter can also be used to
examine similarities and differences between motivational
systems. A persistent theoretical problem in theories of moti-
vation has been specifying the number and form of motiva-
tional processes with which an animal is equipped. We have
suggested that the animal is equipped with as many motiva-
tional systems as there are classes of problems in the environ-
ment for it to solve. We expect that the reader has been struck
by the amount of similarity between the response organiza-
tions proposed to account for feeding and sexual behavior, and
to a lesser extent between those structures and that proposed to
account for the organization of defensive behavior. Each con-
sists of a collection of motivational modes organized by some
kind of imminence continuum. Each includes a set of preexist-
ing stimulus processing and response production tendencies.
The extent to which these similarities are valid remains to be
determined, and this question deserves study. Just as interest-
ing are those disparities between the response organizations.
Appetitive behavior in the feeding behavior system is ex-
tremely flexible. Flexibility in sexually motivated appetitive
behavior has also been demonstrated but is much less well in-
vestigated. In contrast, defensive behavior seems more rigid,
perhaps due to the inherently conservative nature of defense.
The behavioral systems view suggests that motivation is
a much more complex phenomenon than that described by
theories of drive, incentive motivation, or opposing affective


states. Any complete conception must include physiological,
psychological, ecological, and evolutionary factors. Our
approach attempts to address these requirements.

REFERENCES

Akins, C. K., Domjan, M., & Gutierrez, G. (1994). Topography of
sexually conditioned behavior in male Japanese quail (Coturnix
japonica) depends on the CS-US interval. Journal of Experi-
mental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20,199–209.
Alberts, J. R. (1978). Huddling by rat pups: Group behavioral mech-
anisms of temperature regulation and energy conservation. Jour-
nal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 92,231–245.
Aubert, A. (1999). Sickness and behaviour in animals: A motiva-
tional perspective. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 23,
1029–1036.
Balleine, B. (1992). Instrumental performance following a shift in
primary motivation depends upon incentive learning. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 18,
236–250.
Balleine, B., Garner, C., Gonzalez, F. & Dickinson, A. (1995). Moti-
vational control of heterogeneous instrumental chains. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 21,
203–217.
Bandler, R., & Depaulis, A. (1991). Midbrain periaqueductal
gray control of defensive behavior in the cat and rat. In A.
Depauilis & R. Bandler (Eds.), The midbrain periaqueductal
gray matter: Functional, anatomical and immunohistochemical
organization(pp. 175–199). New York: Plenum Press.
Bartholomew, G. A. (1982). Body temperature and energy metabo-
lism. In M. A. Gordon (Ed.), Animal physiology(pp. 333–406).
New York: Macmillan.
Blanchard, R. J., & Blanchard, D. C. (1972). Effects of hippocampal
lesions on the rat’s reaction to a cat. Journal of Comparative &
Physiological Psychology, 78, 77–82.
Blanchard, R. J., & Blanchard, D. C. (1989). Antipredator defensive
behaviors in a visible burrow system. Journal of Comparative
Psychology, 103,70–82.
Blanchard, R. J., Fukunaga, K. K., & Blanchard, D. C. (1976).
Environmental control of defensive reactions to a cat. Bulletin
of the Psychonomic Society, 8,179–181.
Blanchard, R. J., Mast, M., & Blanchard, D. C. (1975). Stimulus
control of defensive reactions in the albino rat. Journal of Com-
parative & Physiological Psychology, 88,81–88.
Blanchard, R. J., Nikulina, J. N., Sakai, R. R., McKittrick, C.,
McEwen, B., & Blanchard, D. C. (1998). Behavioral and en-
docrine change following chronic predatory stress. Physiology &
Behavior, 63,561–569.
Bolhuis, J. J., Hetebrij, E., Den Boer-Visser, A. M., De Groot,
J. H., & Zijlstra, G. G. (2001). Localized immediate early gene
Free download pdf