74 Mood, Cognition, and Memory
To elaborate, consider first the conventional list-learning
paradigm, alluded to earlier. According to Bower and Forgas
(2000, p. 97), this paradigm is ill suited to demonstrating
MDM, because it
arranges only contiguity, not causal belonging, between presen-
tation of the to-be-learned material and emotional arousal. Typi-
cally, the mood is induced minutes before presentation of the
learning material, and the mood serves only as a prevailing back-
ground; hence, the temporal relations are not synchronized to
persuade subjects to attribute their emotional feelings to the ma-
terial they are studying. Thus, contiguity without causal belong-
ing produces only weak associations at best.
In contrast, the model allows for strong mood-dependent
effects to emerge in studies of autobiographical memory,
such as those reported by Eich et al. (1994). Referring to
Figure 3.2, which shows a fragment of a hypothetical asso-
ciative structure surrounding the conceptlake,Bower and
Forgas (2000, pp. 97–98) propose the following:
Suppose [that a] subject has been induced to feel happy and is
asked to recall an incident from her life suggested by the target
wordlake. This concept has many associations including several
autobiographic memories, a happy one describing a pleasantly
thrilling water-skiing episode, and a sad one recounting an
episode of a friend drowning in a lake. These event-memories
are connected to the emotions the events caused. When feeling
happy and presented with the list cue lake,the subject is likely
(by summation of activation) to come up with the water-skiing
memory. The subject will then also associate the list context to
the water-skiing memory and to the word lakethat evoked it.
These newly formed list associations [depicted by dashed lines
in Figure 3.2] are formed by virtue of the subject attributing
causal belonging of the word-and-memory to the experimenter’s
presentation of the item within the list.
These contextual associations are called upon later when the
subject is asked to free recall the prompting words (or the mem-
ories prompted by them) when induced into the same mood or a
different one. If the subject is happy at the time of recall testing,
the water-skiing memory would be advantaged because it would
receive the summation of activation from the happy-mood node
and the list context, thus raising it above a recall level. On the
other hand, if the subject’s mood at recall were shifted to sad-
ness, that node has no connection to the water-skiing memory
that was aroused during list input, so her recall of lakein this
case would rely exclusively upon the association to lake of the
overloaded, list-context node [in Figure 3.2].
Thus, the revised network model, like the AIM, makes a
clear case for choosing autobiographical event generation
over list learning as a means of demonstrating MDM.
Nature of the Retrieval Task
Moreover, both the AIM and the revised network model ac-
commodate an important qualification, which is that mood
dependence is more apt to occur when retention is tested in
the absence than in the presence of specific, observable
LIST
CONTEXT
WATER
SKIING
MEMORY
HAPPY
EMOTION
SADNESS
LAKE EMOTION
FRIEND’S
DROWNING
MEMORY
Figure 3.2 Fragment of a hypothetical person’s associations involving the concept of lake. Lines represent associa-
tions connecting emotion nodes to descriptions of two different events, one happy and one sad. The experimental con-
text becomes associated to experiences that were aroused by cues in that setting. Source:Bower and Forgas, 2000.