Teaching Critical Thinking in Psychology: A Handbook of Best Practices

(ff) #1

205


Why We Believe: Fostering Scientific Thought


Clearly, the problem of comprehensible and accurate presentation of research results is


not always the result only of journalistic flaws. It is no secret that researchers often write


in ways incomprehensible to outsiders. It is also no secret that researchers are sometimes


simply poor writers. As Bruner (1942) wrote, with tongue only partially in her cheek,


“I have even succumbed to a conviction that authors are engaged wilfully and with malice


in suppressing every vestige of spontaneity and emphasis in what they are writing” (p. 53),


including “the tortured circumlocutions of the passive voice” (p. 55). Ferreting through


turgid prose undoubtedly contributes to many journalistic missteps.


Questioning the Conclusions

In one example, several newspapers reported that African-Americans received certain


heart-related treatments only 60% as often as White men. In reality, referrals for Black


men did not differ from those of White men, and Black women were referred 87% as


often as White men. The problem is that journalists misinterpreted a technical term and


misunderstood the research results. As a result, The New York Times, The Washington Post,


and USA Today all misreported the results (Greenstein, 1999).


Because of the different goals of scientific and journalistic writing, readers need to be


aware that the issues that are important to scientists differ from those of journalists. Journalists


look for captivating stories and are probably less interested in the caveats that researchers


think are important. This type of material is useful for students who are learning how to


write their results in either technical or nontechnical format, the demands of which differ.


As an example, reporter Jim Dyer wrote about the so-called “Monster Study” in which


a researcher conditioned children to stutter, some of them experiencing lifelong distress


because of it (Dyer, 2001). Although it was a captivating, if horrific story, researchers sub-


sequently called into question the claims that Dyer made (Ambrose & Yairi, 2002). For


instance, one woman who had participated in the study as a child asserted that her life was


ruined because of her stuttering. It appears that, subsequent to the study, she did not


stutter for the next six decades, beginning to do so only when she met her husband (Owen,


2003b) or when he died (Owen 2003a), depending on the account.


There was further misinformation in The Village Voice (Collins, 2006), the implication


appearing that the researchers unsuccessfully attempted to reverse the stuttering they had


induced. The actual data from the study indicated no increase in stuttering in the groups


that were supposedly conditioned to stutter (Ambrose & Yairi, 2002).


A number of legitimate journalistic sources picked up the story. Unfortunately, the more


scholarly research in a professional journal did not attract much attention. A juicy contro-


versy is always better copy than a sober counterargument. It would behoove students to


learn that news reports about research are always simpler than the actual research and that


it is not wise, particularly with controversial research, to take a news report at face value.


Questioning the Data

Sometimes one can question not only the conclusions that appear in popular sources but


also the data that writers adduce to support their arguments. Best (2001, 2004) has

Free download pdf