Teaching Critical Thinking in Psychology: A Handbook of Best Practices

(ff) #1

Natalie Kerr Lawrence et al.


24


questions for class, and evaluating case studies. Students’ top five activities also included


critiquing a journal article, engaging in debates, and evaluating case studies. For the most


part, students and faculty agreed on which activities were most likely to encourage critical


thinking, r
s


(19) = .84, p < .01. Interestingly, students were more likely to say that a class


activity “always” or “often” helped develop critical thinking, whereas faculty often reported


that the activities “could” encourage critical thinking. Faculty responses acknowledged


that the way an instructor conducts certain class activities is vital to whether that activity


encourages critical thinking.


Our results showed that activities that encourage critical thinking were more likely to occur


in higher level classes. To our surprise, we also found a negative correlation between how likely


an activity was to encourage critical thinking and how likely instructors were to use it in class,


r
s


(19) = −.47, p = .03. This finding may be because activities that encourage critical thinking


can be more difficult to design, implement, and grade than other activities. Indeed, instruc-


tors may be reluctant to incorporate critical thinking activities into their courses because they


perceive the investment to be too costly. In this chapter, we describe specific activities and


techniques that instructors can easily incorporate into any psychology course.


Critical Thinking Framework

Halonen (1995) urged faculty to focus on critical thinking skills and proposed a frame-


work to help “demystify” critical thinking. We used Halonen’s framework to help identify


best practices for fostering critical thinking (see Figure 3.1). The framework includes both


the cognitive and propensity elements of critical thinking. Halpern (2002, 2003) pre-


sented a similar model for teaching critical thinking skills. Halpern recommended learn-


ing activities that:


(a) explicitly teach the skills of critical thinking, (b) develop the disposition for effortful


thinking and learning, (c) direct learning activities in ways that increase the probability of


transcontextual transfer (structure training), and (d) make metacognitive monitoring explicit


and overt. (Halpern, 2003, p. 14)


Like Halonen’s framework, Halpern’s model recognized the importance of both cognitive


and propensity (i.e., dispositional) factors in critical thinking. In addition, both authors


identified metacognition as integral to the development of critical thinking. In the remain-


der of this chapter, we discuss activities designed to address the propensity, cognitive, and


metacognitive components of critical thinking.


Propensity Components

Students who have the skills to think critically do not always use those skills. Like all


thinkers, students are “cognitive misers” (Taylor, 1981) who have neither the ability nor


the motivation to think critically about every issue. Critical thinking takes effort, and

Free download pdf