Teaching Critical Thinking in Psychology: A Handbook of Best Practices

(ff) #1

37


Are They Ready Yet? Developmental Issues


dispositions. Although there are many different taxonomies of important thinking dispo-


sitions available (Ennis, 1987; Facione & Facione, 1995; Perkins et al., 1993) most lists


include traits such as (a) truth-seeking—a desire to know the truth even when it is


unpleasant, (b) intellectual curiosity—an interest in learning for learning’s sake, (c) intel-


lectual humility—a recognition of one’s own limited understanding and information,


(d) open-mindedness—a willingness to consider widely divergent views, (e) trust of


reason—the confidence that reason works, and (f ) intellectual maturity—a tolerance for


ambiguity, for withholding judgment, and for the tentative nature of most knowledge.


There is good evidence that these dispositions are tied to a number of positive educa-


tional outcomes including the use of critical thinking skills (Halpern, 1998; Hofer &


Pintrich, 1997; Klaczynski, 2000; Kuhn & Weinstock, 2002; Sa, Stanovich, & West,


1999). The question is how does one teach these dispositions? Are they even teachable?


How malleable are these attitudes? To this point almost all of the work on critical thinking


dispositions has been theoretical, and little empirical research exists concerning these dis-


positions. However, one of the most promising avenues of theory and research related to


the critical thinking dispositions is in the area of personal epistemology.


Personal Epistemology

From the perspective of the discipline of philosophy, epistemology concerns the origin,


nature, limits, methods, and justification of human knowledge (Hofer, 2002). The study


of personal epistemology, on the other hand, refers to how the individual develops concep-


tions of knowledge—the nature of knowledge, how knowledge is generated, and how one


comes to know what one knows. These issues are implicated in how students study, how


much and in what ways they value their peers in the classroom, what they expect from


faculty, and in their reasons for being in school. Personal epistemology influences compre-


hension, cognitive processing, conceptual change, and learning (Hofer, 2004a). It may be


that differences in personal epistemology are the source of much of faculty frustration in


their attempts to teach higher order thinking skills to students. Not only are students at


different places in their epistemology from each other, but on the whole they are at a very


different place than are faculty. There is often a large disconnection between teachers’


assumptions about knowledge and learning and students’ assumptions. It is an axiom of


educational theory that the most important factor governing what students learn is what


they already know. The research on personal epistemology indicates that what students


believe about what they know may be more important than what they actually know


(Klazsynski, 2000; Sa et al., 1999; Schommer-Aikens & Easter, 2006).


Research into personal epistemology began with the work of Perry (1970) and has grown


extensively in the past 15 years (Moore, 2002). While research in this area has engaged


scholars from a variety of fields, research paradigms, and areas of interest, there are two


primary directions epistemology research has taken (Hofer, 2002). The one direction fol-


lows the Piagetian paradigm of charting a developmental sequence of cognitive changes;


the other approaches personal epistemology as a system of more-or-less independent


beliefs on four or five dimensions that may develop asynchronously (Schommer, 1994).

Free download pdf